WE LIVE IN an age where many millions of people turn to psychics and mediums for help and guidance in so many aspects of their lives. You see the advertisements in nearly every magazine available on every newsagent’s shelf, “call this number for accurate readings”, “love, career, relationships”. The number you dial will either be an 0906 premium rate number where clients are charged at the rate of £1.50 per minute or you can pay by credit card, but you will still pay anything from £30 to £40. Yet so many times what you are told by the psychic you will not be able to relate to, and their predictions in many cases will turn out to be complete nonsense. This kind of disappointment is damaging and distressing to vulnerable individuals who are desperate for help. There may be psychics who genuinely have a desire to help those in need, but I believe they are in the minority.
Is it possible to give someone a psychic reading without having any psychic ability whatsoever? The answer of course is “yes”. Anyone can learn to do psychic readings and earn a very good living in the process. I will show you how to conduct a successful psychic reading later in this article. The only ability you will need is observation.
Another case I know of was a lady
who, during her consultation,
was told the child she was expecting
would be born with considerable
problems. “I can see blood all over
the child,” claimed the psychic.
Three months later her child was
born perfectly healthy
The psychic industry is now a multi-million pound industry all around the world and yet most magicians and mentalists are able to do what psychics do and, in many cases, do it better. Some people mistakenly consult psychics every time a problem crops up in their lives. If that psychic turns out to give wrong information, they will then consult another one in the hope that they will hear what they are looking for.
I know of a lady who consulted a psychic for a one day reading and was told that her husband might not see his next birthday. This dreadful information turned out to be wholly inaccurate but the worry and fear that this instilled into this poor woman was inexcusable. Another case I know of was a lady who, during her consultation, was told the child she was expecting would be born with considerable problems. “I can see blood all over the child,” claimed the psychic. Three months later her child was born perfectly healthy. Again, the distress this caused was enormous. It would be wrong to tar all psychics with the same brush. Some are very accurate in their utterances.
Sadly, they are few and far between. On the whole, more people gain nothing by consulting psychics, than those who do gain. An explanation of how a reading can be done without any psychic ability: It is always safe to assume that anyone who seeks out a psychic has a problem that they are in quest of an answer for. If it is a lady, it is often a safe bet she has a relationship problem. The golden rule is observation. For example, if you say to a client “You’ve been very upset recently”, more often than not they will be helped because you are right. If you take note of their reaction, it will always give you the answer. Go on to say, “This is to do with a man, isn’t it?” Nine out of ten times you will be correct.
By this time, your client is convinced you are psychic because you are spot on and all you’ve done is made a safe guess and observed their reaction. In other words, they have unknowingly given you information but they are convinced that you have given it to them.
A trick that seldom fails: You can then go on to say “Ah well, never mind, brighter times are on their way. This time next year, you will be wonderfully happy.”
Then you have created a happy optimism within the client. I am well aware that this practice of deceit is despicable and should not happen, but it does, and sadly many of the deceived will go back for more.
Then there are so-called psychics who really believe they have a gift but never really prove anything with it. Just before my wife and I met, she visited a medium who is now very well known throughout the United Kingdom. My wife had just lost her first husband in a road traffic accident and was hoping for proof that he was OK on the other side. The medium didn’t even mention this, which at the time of her reading was a huge sadness in her life. When he reached the end of the reading, he invited her to ask him a question, “I’ve just lost my husband” she said. “Can you tell me if he is OK on the other side?” “Oh yes, I feel a stabbing”. Wrong. “I know he was with you when he died, wasn’t he?” Wrong again. He went on to mention several more possibilities of how he may have died, none of which were true. It was plain that he was guessing in the hope that he would hit on the reason, but he didn’t. “Oh, he’s OK on the other side and sends you his love.” My wife left the reading feeling worse than she did before going there. The sad fact is that so many people look to psychics and depend on them to get out of bed in the morning. Well, perhaps that’s a slight exaggeration, but very few of those who have regular psychic readings are aware of the tricks of the trade, so to speak. Many argue that great comfort is gained from readings and maybe that’s no bad thing. It should also be made known to the sitter that 90% of the time the information they are given is not from the spirit world. I recently sat through a public demonstration of mediumship where the medium was saying things like, “I’ve got an Irish connection here and this man came from a big family”. It was odd to me that the information being given, he gave to several other people in the same audience on the same evening. Other comments, such as, “I’ve got a man here that passed with a heart attack”. Don’t we all know someone, or know of someone, who died of a heart attack?
Many psychics are decent, giving people who would never intentionally hurt anyone. There is both self-delusion and deliberate fraud. I think most psychics fall into the former category and not the latter. It’s time to challenge them and show to them it is just not real. Having worked as a psychic myself, I know how easy it is to fool yourself into believing you are possessed by wonderful powers and gifts. I realise after years of indecision that I had to move on and take a very different view of the whole psychic scene. For over five years, I felt ill at ease with the work I was doing. I remember some years ago being invited to appear on a television programme called Head to Head. I was on as a psychic, and sitting in the opposite chair was a well known magician and sceptic, Ian Rowland. The idea was that he would do a fake reading for someone in the studio audience and I would do a genuine reading, with the audience being led to believe we were both genuine psychics.
The conclusion was that Ian’s reading was better than mine. When the presenter told the audience that Ian was a magician, they were gob-smacked, to say the least. On another occasion I was invited to be in the audience during a recording of James Randi Investigates at Granada Television. Each week he would look at various types of psychic practices and generally try to disprove them. At the time I thought that James was speaking from the wrong end of his person. With hindsight, I think the psychics taking part offered no real evidence whatsoever. What the majority of mediums put forward as evidence just doesn’t stand up to scrutiny. Psychics must really start to be brutally honest with themselves. Although I found Mr Randi’s approach rather harsh and hurtful, I do agree with his conclusions about psychics. I am confident that no-one will ever win the big cash challenge Mr Randi has on offer. Of all the television programmes I’ve ever been involved with or seen other psychics on, not yet has a psychic triumphed over a magician. It worries me that people are paying fortunes for what, in so many cases, turns out to be rubbish.
James Byrne worked as a psychic for thirty years before coming to the conclusion that it was all a sham. During his time as a psychic, he toured New Zealand twice, hosted radio shows in the United Kingdom, and performed at the London Palladium. He now earns his living as a writer and debunker of psychics.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 20, Issue 3, from 2007
You can buy nearly anything on eBay. From tractors to tricycles, from lip-gloss to liposuction cream – if it’s legally available, then you can purchase it on the world’s favourite auction site.
Not only is eBay one of the best places to make the odd penny from your unwanted Christmas gifts or the antiques you’ve recently rediscovered in your attic, it is also a useful means of a steady tax-free income for thousands of people all over the UK. Artists whose work might never get further than the local gallery can now exhibit and sell nationally and authors whose self-published books were only ever available on a handful of bookshop shelves can now get worldwide distribution on eBay.
Amongst those who make a profit from auctioning their wares are the psychic community, who offer a whole range of spiritual delights, ranging from one question readings, to in-depth past life dossiers, all done never having even met their ‘clients’ or touched or seen anything belonging to them or their dearly departed loved ones.
Of course telephone readings have been a source of income for lone spiritualists, along with those larger companies who specialise in making a profit from the pain of the bereaved, for quite some time now. At the back of most women’s magazines the reader will always find, between the ads for plastic surgery and abortion, columns of psychic phone-lines offering their advice and assistance.
However, the beauty of eBay is that it is far cheaper and easier to control. An advertisement in the back of a top woman’s magazine, for example, will cost in the hundreds, yet an online auction listing is less than fifty pence. Also, the seller can plan the exact time and date that their ‘item’ finishes on eBay, therefore knowing just when they will be asked for the reading (payment permitting).
Another aspect of eBay is the ‘Buy It Now’ feature, where sellers can place multiple items for purchase at a set price. On the basis of this, a small time publisher could market twenty copies of his book in one single advert at a relatively cheap listing fee, or an astute psychic sell as many readings or tarot spreads as they wish at a fixed price of their choosing.
Over the past few months I have personally purchased several readings from a range of different spiritualists on eBay, in order to compile a small amount of data on exactly what is on offer and, more importantly, precisely what it is that people are buying. Having collected a certain quantity of information, I then discovered, with great interest, that not only are psychic readings readily available on eBay, but so too are electronic ebooks with exact instructions on how to make a profit from tarot cards and other such spiritual money-makers.
The Electronic Tarot
An extremely interesting eBay purchase of mine was a digital download bought from an American seller listed as “MAKE $ DOING PSYCHIC TAROT CARD READINGS”. The download (entitled Psychic Reading Money Machine) comes with a complete tarot reading programme and instructions as to how to design, list and sell the reading on eBay. Costing only 73p (a lot cheaper than £100 for a ‘Mediumship Course’, also offered on eBay), the blurb forthrightly informs its reader that:
After running the auction, you need to be prompt in dealing with each customer individually … do your reading via the computer generated tarot and then write it in your own words. Don’t copy the reading word for word as it needs to read like an authentic reading and not some generated one.
It continues by posing the question “How much money can you actually make?” and answers it with the following information:
A lot of people who sell this type of service on eBay usually charge $15.00 per reading and sell 15 – 20 readings per week, which generates around $300 a week.
The actual computer tarot consists of entering the virtual sitters name and date of birth. It will then give a ten-card spread, which features pictures of the cards and fairly detailed and informed descriptions, under the headings ‘Your Power Cards’, ‘Your Desire Cards’, ‘Your Core Cards’, ‘Your Growth Cards’ and ‘Your Lucky Cards’.
The electronic programme goes on to inform its reader that, when listing such items on eBay, it is:
… best to offer a selection. Services such as Numerology, Natal reports, Biorhythms and Compatibility readings, all of which are unique, would be excellent sellers.
After which it offers such complementary programmes at a reasonable cost. The final words of advice from the Psychic Reading Money Machine are:
Mispelled (sic) words are ok while doing a reading. A real reading is not of perfection but a reading of personal guidance. If it is to (sic) textbook, it will not seem authentic.
Of course, the computer-generated cards are very similar to buying a set of tarot cards and an instruction book and producing a spread, then looking the meanings up in the book, typing them into a credible format and emailing this to the buyer. Nevertheless, by using the computer it is incredibly quick and easy and, by asking for a name and date of birth, it seems more authentic. However, whatever name (male or female) is typed into the computer-generated tarot, if the date of birth is the same, it will always produce identical cards and explanations.
The World’s Psychic Marketplace
There are a variety of readings available on offer on eBay (around five hundred at any given time) and, in sifting through them and making my selection for purchase, I tried to be as diverse as possible in what I chose. The price scale of readings ranges from very inexpensive (around two pounds) to the more extravagant (£150 for a comprehensive yearly psychic forecast).
My first buy was from a spiritualist who had only just started to sell his goods on the Internet and was, due to that, very cheap. I paid 99p for a very detailed reading, from his ‘Ancient form of Irish Celtic Reading’ assisted by his ‘spiritual guide’.
The content of the reading dealt with the usual – money, health, relationships and emotions. The use of cold reading was extremely evident and everything addressed could have been applied to anyone (‘you need to stop people putting you down – be more sure and assert yourself in a nice way’). The names presented in the reading were very obvious ones, such as John, Anne, Paul, Joanne and Clair.
The reading ended with ‘Many blessing, all my love’ followed by the offer of answering any more questions that I might have via postal reading, telephone or email, along with promoting candles ‘that are made and blessed by the spirits’. Another eBay reading I chose was ‘One Question Only – Thoth Reading’. Here I was able to ask the spirit world any question in exchange for the winning price of the auction, along with supplying my full name and date of birth. The question I posed was ‘Is anyone in the spirit world trying to contact me?’ An hour later I received an email regretfully telling me that the medium was unable to answer this question (although for more money she would be very happy to do so) and that I could either ask the spirits another question or get a refund.
It is interesting to view what else the psychics are selling on eBay whilst simultaneously offering the readings. A tarot reading purchased on a ‘Buy it Now’ auction was from an ‘in house psychic’ of a company who specialised in detox pads. Here a five-card spread was used in answer to a question, and written in six precise paragraphs. A week later I purchased another tarot reading from the same seller and received (although a different spread of cards) exactly the same formatted and paragraphed structure of equal length. Both readings, neatly presented and well written, strikingly resembled the composition of the computer-generated tarot readings.
eBay uses a ‘feedback’ system in order for users to assess whether or not they want to purchase an item from a seller. For example, if a member has had a bad experience with a purchase, they can leave negative feedback and this information can be accessed by other users. In browsing through the list of psychics selling tarot spreads, healing and past life readings, it is insightful to peruse what others who have bought from them have had to say.
A certain lady psychic who refers to herself as a ‘Famous TV Medium’ on eBay, was selling readings at £15.00 a time (‘Buy It Now’), until she received negative feedback from her clients. Within the space of a month, comments were appearing on her feedback page such as ‘Sorry but the reading was unclear and not usefulat all for me’, ‘Not brilliant, expected more …’ and ‘DO NOT BID!! Total rubbish plagiarised from AOL’s horoscope – didn’t even change words.’
After these unconstructive observations, the medium in question was forced to lower her price to £5.00, before gaining enough positive feedback to enable her to put the cost back up again in time for Christmas.
Image is Everything
Description, layout and image are extremely important when marketing a product on eBay and, when there are four hundred and ninety-nine other sellers all trading the same sort of product, it is crucial that yours stands apart from the rest.
eBay psychics tend to utilise a range of images to promote themselves and their goods. Some will use their own photographs, others pictures of beautiful blond women and others still illustrations of tarot cards. Descriptions are varied, as are the titles, but generally always include the words such as ‘authentic’, ‘genuine’, ‘honest’, ‘spirit guide’ and ‘destiny’.
With such tough competition, sellers rely heavily on feedback and sometimes use their positive comments in their item description. However, as with the marketing of specific products, there are certain colours and images associated with spiritualism and these are sometimes also employed by the psychic sellers. The popular spiritualist colour blue features on quite a few of the auction adverts, along with images of crystal balls, Native American Indians, wolves, angels and rainbows. Some sellers go further in their personal descriptions, as the following illustrates.
I am a practising Witch, mother and grandmother, attuned to Nature and the power of the elements and a professional Psychic and am well skilled with several forms of divination techniques.
Another description of a psychic reading included the statement:
Just to be clear on what to expect … don’t expect, just be wiling to except help from your silent witness with the view that this is purely and simply a prediction and foresight as what your guides have seen as the most probable outcomes.
The use of the word ‘counselling’ is also an effective means of selling. Tarots, rune castings and spiritual readings focussing specifically on love are also seen to be good sellers, with mediums asking for pictures of both their buyer and the loved one in question, along with some background information on the situation.
Information on the particular psychic is also sometimes available on the auction description but, interestingly those who call themselves a ‘TV medium’ rarely state the programmes they have featured in. However, one particular eBay clairvoyant is all too happy to inform his readers that he is a ‘famous Polish TV celebrity and fortune teller’ whose TV career started with the Polish edition of Big Brother. His eBay biog continues by telling his readers that he:
… predicted the outcome of the last presidential election in Poland and was hired by politicians to help them predict what would happen to them in that time …
The astounding life story doesn’t stop there as his spiel continues, telling his readers that he has:
…read tarot cards to many TV, theatre and show business celebrities around the world (as an example Steven Segal, the Polish premiere and others).
However, amazingly there is absolutely no mention of this ‘famous fortune teller’ when his name is typed into a search engine such as Google, apart from his eBay listings and a snippet of a book review that he has written, where he proudly calls himself ‘The Only Polish Famous Ventriloquist and Mindbender’(sic).
Virtual Belief
Readings via email are not incredibly recent and some prolific mediums have been using this form of communication with clients for quite some time. Craig Hamilton-Parker, for example, began offering a similar service from his website some years ago, although he now only gives one-to-one consultations, but still recommends a link to a very similar site (endorsed by him) where online psychics proffer readings via email or telephone.
In today’s society, where nearly everything can be accessed through a PC and where we find ourselves increasingly isolated from the outside world because of this, what better way than to connect with the spirits than via the Internet? Instead of having to turn out to visit the spiritualist church on a rainy Sunday evening, or attend a psychic meeting at the local community centre on a blustery winter’s afternoon, eBay provides a whole range of mediums offering a varying selection of spiritual amusements for the susceptible masses.
However much humankind relies on computers and technology and, due to this, inadvertently find themselves distanced from true personal contact, one thing is clear – the need to be loved and receive love in return, along with the desire to believe in the life hereafter burns as strongly as ever and, depending on just how much money you have to squander, anyone can be spiritually reassured, comforted and counselled by the simple click of a mouse.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 20, Issue 3, from 2007
Some recent dialogue with a hard-line fellow sceptical physicist has started me thinking about the pros and cons of the internet for people peddling paranormal and pseudoscientific theories.
Specifically, many years ago I appeared on the BBC TV programme, Pebble Mill at One to counter the claims of one John Searl that he had invented and constructed an anti-gravity, perpetual motion flying machine. If I remember correctly, an unfortunate fire at his premises meant that he could not actually demonstrate a functioning vehicle to the cameras; however, he described how his machine (based on rotating magnets) was self-powering and, in a shower of electrical sparks, had been observed by witnesses to levitate and take off into the distance. He also had photographs of mock-ups of his vehicles.
According to Searl, it would only require a little refinement to the technology to enable us to journey to the moon and beyond in a vehicle that today would have the advantage of contributing nothing to global carbon emissions.
I took a sceptical but fairly soft line with the self-styled Professor Searl and did not cast any aspersions on either his integrity or his sanity. Afterwards, my physicist colleague took me strongly to task for my soft line, reckoning that I had “let him off the hook” by using analogies with children’s toys and a Wimshurst machine in an attempt to explain Searl’s purported observations regarding his ‘flying’machines.
The same colleague has recently been back in touch to tell me that John Searl has gone from strength to strength over the years and now has three websites (searleffect.com, searlsolution.com, swallowcommand.com) dedicated to his electromagnetic Searl Effect Generator (S.E.G.) as well as many clips on the video-hosting website YouTube. To add insult to injury, Searl includes images taken from the Pebble Mill programme — including one or two of me (incorrectly titled “a physicist from Manchester University”) in the “media” section of one of the websites, presumably trying to give the impression that I am one of his supporters.
Having taken a look at Searl’s websites, it is certainly true that his activities, and those of similar ‘inventors’ as well as purveyors of pseudoscientific theories and paranormal belief systems in general, have a global reach via the web that they definitely didn’t have back in the days of self-published pamphlets. But does the tremendous opportunity for self-promotion that the web offers necessarily imply that inventors like Searl will be taken more seriously by the general public?
There are undoubtedly contexts where this is the case. A professionally-produced website can certainly serve to lend credibility to many dubious activities. But, by the same token, the detailed exposure of ‘unconventional’ theories using amateur film footage that allows the inventor to explain in his own words the novelty and originality of his ideas can also serve to diminish the credibility of the invention.
Some of the clips on Searl’s website have him dressed in a doctoral gown over what I am informed is an Air Vice Marshall’s uniform explaining the theory behind his rotating magnets. I am not convinced that exposure to this information will have BAe Systems, NASA, or Electricité de France queuing up to invest in the S.E.G.
In short, by allowing ‘unconventional’ inventors to demonstrate their wares at great length, the internet may well be providing a service to scepticism rather than the opposite.
My colleague strongly exhorts me to threaten the publishers of the website with litigation in order to get them to remove images of me from their site – but for the moment this ‘physicist from Manchester University’ feels he has more useful things to do with his time.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 20, Issue 3, from 2007
It is easy to be alarmist about education. Education, after all, bears directly on something in which everyone is strongly invested: the shape of things to come, the future of society and all that.
We have all, at some time, heard anecdotes about what students are learning or failing to learn in contemporary educational institutions which have made us gasp, chuckle or shake our heads. For teachers, though, some of their most powerful and satisfying teaching emerges when they encounter and redress exactly such outrageous gaps or errors in their students’ knowledge.
My alarm was aroused on many occasions as a secondary teacher, and in every subject I taught, from history through to literature, but nowhere was it as profoundly felt as in the Religious Education classes I was allotted in an Anglican Boys School in Melbourne.
My complaint was not the traditional one that might be expected within this subject. The issue was not with a lack of student familiarity with the Christian or Western tradition, still less was it a problem with overwhelming faithlessness. Quite the contrary: my fears were aroused by the breadth and inconsistency of what my students believed or half-believed in, and in the seemingly accidental character of their beliefs.
Beneath their adolescent front of weary scepticism, my students subscribed to an eclectic belief in: ghosts, demons, exorcism, astrology, witchcraft, alien abduction, telekinesis, telepathy, clairvoyance, precognition… the list goes on. What is more, classes between the ages of 14 and 18 showed little variation in the degree, quantity or sophistication of their supernatural beliefs and showed a similar cluster of historical and scientific naiveties. In other words, little development along these lines seemed to be taking place either privately or through the normal process of schooling.
What became rapidly clear to me was that most students had not personally reflected on their beliefs at length or discussed them in an organized way. In fact, they did not feel their beliefs worthy of such analysis. It also became clear that in the absence of any rigorous critical filter teenagers were prone to tepidly accept whatever portraits of reality were presented to them, be they even the most fantasy-infiltrated films or television serials.
Science-fiction and fantasy, once fringe genres, have now become, for whatever reasons, dominant and central entertainment fare, especially for children and teenagers; as a consequence, the same combinations of half-belief and half-wanting-to-believe that afflicted many of the former subscribers seem to have become endemic. As a mass phenomenon, however, these tendencies seem to be less explicit and less sophisticated than before. The rich allegorical dimension of sci-fi and fantasy is being persistently downgraded or ignored by a public overwhelmingly drawn to the sensational aspects of these genres.
In response to these issues, I embarked with my classes on a sustained and evolving discussion of the limits of ‘the possible’. We examined scientifically chartered domains of uncanny psychosomatic phenomena: hypnotism, placebo, and Harvard studies of Yogic and Sufi mediation. We explored some of the spectacular supernatural effects achievable by illusionists and their dubious application by charlatans in parts of India. We discussed the multiple interpretations of contentious religious terms such as ‘miracle’. We investigated the diversity of explanations for the miraculous events in the Christian tradition. Lastly, we explored the witchtrials of Salem and Loudon. Here we observed the very real and terrible dangers which can arise when the political and legal mechanisms of society are infected with superstitious prejudices and how readily the unscrupulous can exploit such prejudices to their profit.
It is these latter issues which most closely touch upon the sources of my alarm and concern. Irrational and dangerous superstitious fervor is not a thing of the past, nor is it exclusive to less developed countries; far from it. As a democratic citizen I feel threatened by the political implications of unchecked credulity and superstition in the public I share, or will soon share, my vote with, suspecting, as Voltaire said, “those who can make you believe absurdities can make you commit atrocities.” It is inevitable that supernatural and religious beliefs will affect the democratic process. What we must ensure is that citizens have scrutinized or are capable of scrutinizing those beliefs.
As a teacher it was immediately gratifying to watch groups which were for the most part hostile, inert and poorly motivated, become engaged, open and dynamic. Clearly there was a latent hunger to discuss this material.
It was amazing to find that students who had started the course believing that Indian fakirs could levitate, some weeks later were hovering anywhere from a few inches to a few feet off the classroom carpet; laughing as they competed to outdo their classmates in illusionist tricks they had independently researched and mastered. A few students had even naively believed that certain stage magicians’ feats of levitation were paranormal, which shows just how difficult the distinction between suspended disbelief and actual belief can be for some youngsters.
The questioning and discussion our topics generated would often spill out into the playground at break times. I sometimes overheard philosophical discussions about whether, say, unconscious pain was possible, being carried over from classes weeks beforehand.
My greatest satisfaction in all of this, though, was the evidence I consistently gleaned that whatever beliefs the students subscribed to, they were leaving each class a little better equipped not only to assess the fantastic components of the entertainment they consumed, but to encounter and resist the predations of cults, charlatans and demagogues claiming to have supernatural powers or authority.
Compulsory education is the principal domain within which we combat the coercive threats democracy is subject to, and thus fostering an astute and informed public. A reliable and responsible media is equally necessary. We must do our best to ensure that these two institutions collaborate to guarantee our citizens a common, rational understanding of the world, an understanding unthreatened by conditioned gullibility or superstitious atavism.
The shapers of contemporary youth culture (and, by default, the future of culture) seem to suggest that the antidote to disenchantment is some kind of mythological regression. They present angels, witchcraft and aliens as the solution to confusion, anomie and alienation. They have a good reason for making this sales-pitch, though: we buy it. The engineers of youth culture are, in the end, corporations relentlessly seeking the teenage dollar. In their fierce competition for adolescent attention, they know that the spectacular or mysterious will nearly always triumph over the mundane. Furthermore, the massive resources at their disposal make their messages infinitely more seductive than those that parents or teachers promulgate.
With this in mind we must question how freely we can allow the purely mercenary logic of the entertainment and advertising industries to dictate how children see the world, especially when manufacturing a malleable public serves the immediate interests of corporations hoping to make us buy things we don’t need.
Beyond this, at every stage and in every discipline within compulsory and even tertiary education I feel we are called to alert students to the vast civilized and scholarly apparatus which encounters and accounts for uncanny phenomena, providing natural rather than supernatural explanations and causes. It is the half-cherished belief that there is some unexplored dark-side to culture where civilization fears to tread which nourishes contemporary superstition.
When uncanny phenomena are explored from a historical, anthropological, psychological or broadly scientific perspective the ‘super’-natural vanishes. The amazement, awe and wonder, however, usually remain.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 20, Issue 1, from 2007.
THE GREAT MASTURBATION delusion of the 19th century was exceptional in that it involved extraordinary behaviour not on the part of panicking mobs or obsessed fanatics, but of physicians and scientists of the highest repute, moral and religious leaders of unimpeachable standing. Weighted with their authority, the delusion infected countless lesser minds that accepted and repeated their dictates.
The delusion consisted of the belief that masturbation is uniquely harmful to those who practise it, leading inexorably to feebleness and debility through a wide assortment of ailments to madness, and ultimately death. The delusion was subscribed to, and often vehemently promoted by, men and women in the highest scientific positions throughout Western Europe and North America, as well as churchmen and others with the moral clout to ensure that they would be listened to with respect. Yet in support of these affirmations there was not a scrap of scientific evidence. Instead, scientific and medical findings were distorted and manipulated to give a wholly erroneous impression. As a result, millions of children were subjected to a reign of terror in which horrific mental suffering was often accompanied by physical torture.
Masturbation is practised throughout the world, in almost every known culture. “It is found among the people of nearly every race of which we have an intimate knowledge, however natural the conditions under which man and woman may live,” sexologist Havelock Ellis declared more than a century ago, and he provided examples showing that among Africans “no secret is made of it… it is treated as one of the most ordinary facts of life. Throughout the East masturbation is very prevalent, especially among young girls…” and so on (Ellis, 1924). Culturally, the area in which the delusion was promulgated was almost wholly Judeo-Christian, and the teachings of the Jewish and Christian religions formed its basis. In particular, they taught that any sexual activity outside marriage is sinful, particularly when – as with masturbation – it is non-procreative. The pleasure that accompanies sexual activity, being simply a biological device to provide an incentive, was not to be sought for its own sake. Consequently, masturbation always ranked high on the list of forbidden acts.
In practice, however, this was little more than a technicality. During the centuries previous to the 18th, the taboo on masturbation, insofar as any taboo existed, was wholly a theological one, stemming from what the Church believed were God’s wishes in the matter. Doctors saw no reason to stigmatise masturbation on medical grounds; at most they would discourage excess, as with any other form of sexual activity (Ellis, 1924). Indeed, the eminent 16th century anatomist Falloppio encouraged parents to stimulate their son’s penis on the grounds that repeated erections would make it larger and thus capable of giving greater pleasure to a future wife (Ellis, 1924).
Science considers masturbation
At the beginning of the 18th century, however, the taboo acquired a supposedly scientific dimension, with the publication of writings purporting to demonstrate the deleterious consequences of masturbation. The ball was set rolling by the 1710 publication, by an anonymous English author (plausibly identified by Laqueur, 2002, as hack writer John Marten) of Onania, or the heinous sin of self-pollution. Though, as its title suggests, it was the moral aspects of the matter which were the focus, his book echoed the spirit of the Age of Enlightenment in finding a ‘scientific’ dimension for the moral teaching: masturbation was identified and soundly denounced as a serious medical hazard.
Though Marten had no professional standing, his book was immensely popular and led to more serious discussion. However, it is doubtful whether the delusion would have taken firm root without the authority of a recognised professional. One of those who had been profoundly influenced by the English book – which was widely translated – was a Swiss physician of high repute, Samuel-August Tissot, who in 1758 published (at first in Latin, later in French) L’onanisme, ou dissertation physique sur les maladies produites par la masturbation. (Incidentally, though both these authors employ the term ‘onanism’ as though it were synonymous with masturbation, it is nothing of the sort. The crime of Onan, a biblical figure, was to spill his seed upon the ground rather than impregnate his sister-inlaw. Not only is masturbation not condemned in the Bible, it is not as much as mentioned.)
Following these two very influential books, the dangers of masturbation were supposed to be built upon an ostensibly solid medical foundation. Masturbation was perceived as responsible for a wide range of ailments, ranging from digestive disorders to deteriorating eyesight, and leading to insanity. For Tissot, the masturbator was a criminal whose condition “more justly entitles him to the contempt than the pity of his fellow creatures” (Ellis, 1924). Moreover – and this was to be a key factor in the creation of the delusion – Tissot purported to show that masturbation was more harmful to the individual than ‘normal’ forms of sexual activity. His book sold in vast quantities and was widely translated. His great reputation ensured that his statements carried authority. Though his case histories were dubious and his reasoning faulty, he appeared to be providing abundant evidence that masturbation led to degeneration of the individual, culminating in madness and death.
From then on, the chorus of condemnation of masturbation increased in volume throughout the rest of the 18th century, reaching a crescendo in the 19th century. Attitudes were similar throughout Europe and North America: in 1780, the American doctor Johann Frank insisted that onanism had become so widespread in American schools that the authorities could not take too much care to stamp out this plague. The medical-scientific aspects of the subject were accepted without question, the only differences of opinion being as to how many maladies should be attributed to the practice.
By the early 19th century, virtually all authorities accepted the belief as fact. Benjamin Rush, the most respected physician in America, asserted in 1812 that masturbation led to a whole range of ailments including pulmonary consumption, dimness of sight, vertigo, epilepsy, loss of memory, and ultimately death. In France, the 1819 Dictionnaire des sciences medicales confidently claimed that “the continual excitement of the genital organs is liable to give rise to almost all the acute or chronic illnesses which can disturb the harmony of our functions”. Diagnosis became a self-fulfilling process. Since masturbation led to so many ailments, clearly anyone suffering from any of these ailments was probably guilty of masturbation: and since almost every patient admitted to masturbating at one time or another, the sequence of cause and effect was self-evident. The list of believers included many of the greatest names in medicine. In France, the eminent Jean-Etienne-Dominique Esquirol took it for granted that no respectable medical authority could doubt the harmfulness of the practice: “Masturbation is recognized in all countries as a common cause of insanity… by lowering the powers of resistance it reduces the patient to a state of stupidity, to phtisis, marasmus, and death”. The idea took root in England in the 1820s, and in Germany in the 1830s. Doctors everywhere echoed the view of French physician Reveille-Parise who in 1828 declared: “In my opinion, neither plague, nor war, nor smallpox, nor a crowd of similar evils, have resulted more disastrously for humanity than the habit of masturbation: it is the destroying element of civilized society” (cited in Laqueur, 2002).
However, as Lesley Hall has shown (2003), the matter was not quite so simple. She demonstrates that the delusion presented significant changes of emphasis. At first, following Onania and Tissot, the emphasis was on the physical consequences of masturbation. This emphasis gradually shifted to what it did to the mind, driving the patient insane. As the 19th century progressed, the masturbatory hypothesis became more specifically applied to madness. Some doctors, among them the distinguished Henry Maudsley, identified specific types of insanity, which could be linked to masturbation. To his credit, Maudsley subsequently withdrew his 1868 assertions, but for a while ‘masturbatory insanity’ was an essential element of diagnosis, explaining a wide range of psychiatric conditions.
Male voices outweighed female voices here as elsewhere, but masturbation was no less vilified by women: the eminent American doctor Elizabeth Blackwell saw masturbation as the precursor of “all other forms of unnatural vice”, and feminist writer Mary Wollstonecraft considered “private vices” (she clearly intended masturbation) to be “a public pest”. Towards the end of the 19th century masturbation came to be associated with ‘neurasthenia’ rather than madness, and for those who continued to condemn it in the 20th century it was regarded rather as a psychological defect.
However, these were trends of emphasis rather than clear-cut distinctions: underlying them there persisted the almost unquestioned conviction that self-abuse (as it was now widely labelled) was one of humankind’s most terrible scourges and must be eradicated by whatever means offered themselves.
Sanctioned by the medical authorities, popular authors felt it their duty to echo their judgment in terms accessible to the general public. Thus the American self-appointed ‘Professor’ Fowler let loose in 1875 with a diatribe whose exaggerated terms bear witness to the well-nigh unbelievable extremes to which the masturbation delusion carried those who subscribed to it: “Neither Christendom nor heathendom suffers any evil at all to compare with this… Pile all other evils together – drunkenness upon all cheateries, swindlings, robberies and murders, and tobacco upon both, and all sickness, diseases and pestilence upon all, and war as the cap-sheaf of them all – and all combined cause not a tithe as much human deterioration and misery as does this secret sin”.
To stigmatise a schoolboy or girl enjoying a solitary orgasm as worse than the ravages of the Huns might seem extreme, but such expressions of horror were commonplace. The prolific Dr Rengade, author of many excellent books of popular information, can be taken as representative of popular European authors. This is his take on masturbation – “the most shameful of all vices”, in his 1881 book La vie normale:
A frivolous conversation, the reading of a book, the sight of an attractive person, sometimes a single word, suffices to excite these burning souls, to inspire erotic dreams whose inevitable consequence can only be a spontaneous pollution or one brought on by masturbation. Defying all surveillance, the adolescents seek solitude: they hide, without waiting for nightfall which is most favourable to these vile manoeuvres, and may indeed succeed, simply by friction of the thighs, to satisfy their shameful passion even beneath the eyes of parents or teachers. Miserable beings, of whatever sex, who, deficient in willpower, abandon themselves to these superficial joys, who cannot live without these sorry pleasures! Soon it becomes a veritable mania which drives them to devote themselves to it. The brain exhausts itself in unhealthy overexcitement which the organs, overtaxed, refuse to obey. Haggard, panting, the wretched creature struggles to provoke the voluptuous spasms which lead only to fatigue. Soon, the eyes grow ringed and lifeless in their sockets, the lips hang flabby, the nostrils become pinched, the features grow to resemble a monkey’s rather than a human’s, the head droops in shame, the shoulders are bowed, the limbs become emaciated: and these first signs are swiftly followed by more serious symptoms – phthisis, epilepsy, hysteria, imbecility, madness and consumption.
The remedies proposed by the good doctor were incessant surveillance; early rising; going to bed late when sleep will come swiftly; daily baths and cold showers; severe diet; gymnastic exercises, manual tasks, and exhausting country walks, all backed by remonstrance and reproaches from the parents.
As a physician, Rengade was concerned only with the physical consequences of the practice, but in Britain and America it was the interweaving of science with morals that gave the delusion a double force. The bestselling Sylvanus Stall can be taken as representative in this regard. His What a young boy ought to know sold in the hundreds of thousands and was widely translated:
If you were ever to fall a victim to this vice… you would begin to lose faith in all that is good, and as you persisted in your sin, you would grow less and less like Jesus, and more and more like Satan.
But of course he also points out, at some length, the physical consequences: the masturbator:
…gradually drops back towards the foot of his class… he no longer has his accustomed pleasure in the vigorous romp, the hearty laugh, the good fellowship which characterizes a boy with a vigorous mind and a strong body… the health gradually declines. The eyes lose their lustre. The skin becomes sallow. The muscles become flabby. Every little effort is followed by weariness. Work becomes distasteful and irksome. He complains of pain in the back, of headache and dizziness. The hands become cold and clammy. The digestion becomes poor, the appetite fitful. He sits in a stooping position, becomes hollow-chested, and the entire body becomes wasted, and many signs give promise of early decline and death.
How the public responded to this teaching and preaching can only be surmised from scraps of information that slip past the taboo, which ensured that the subject was rarely mentioned except in the form of warnings. Many masturbators carried on covertly, no doubt. The historian of the MORZINE outbreak mentions, quite casually, that the country girls would meet for group masturbation. At more sophisticated social levels, diaries and autobiographical narratives bear witness to the torment of moral guilt and physical terror induced by the likes of Rengade and Stall.
Eradicating self-abuse
Along with the identification of masturbation as the root cause of so many ailments, came discussion as to how it could be eradicated. Moral condemnation could go only so far with an impetuous youth: so more practical means had to be found. In simple cases, prevention was tried before cure. Little girls should be discouraged from riding hobbyhorses, boys from sliding down stair banisters. The bicycle and the treadle sewing machine were recognised as dangerous. Vigorous activity just before bedtime, leaving the child too fatigued to indulge his/her vice, was advised. Cold baths were prescribed. François Raspail, in his manual of health (1845) recommended that the child’s genitals should be wrapped in a heavy layer of camphor powder, and it should be sprinkled on the bed sheets or between the mattress and the sheets before putting the child to bed. The child could even wear a bathing suit with a bag of camphor strategically placed. The hands could be tied to the bedpost. Ingenious devices were designed, such as enclosing a spiked cage to enclose the penis, which became uncomfortable if erection occurred. ‘Chastity belts’ of various types were available for boys and girls.
One rather surprising solution had been proposed early in the 18th century: Bernard de Mandeville, a Dutch physician practising in England, in A modest defence of public stews, proposed the establishment of authorised brothels, providing a healthier outlet for young men than masturbation. Though moral objections prevented the idea from being accepted, other later opponents of masturbation would discreetly advocate this solution, though it failed to address the equally alarming propensity of females to the vice.
Surgery was also advocated. About 1858, Dr Isaac Baker Brown, a prominent London surgeon, proposed clitoridectomy – the surgical removal of the clitoris – for female masturbators, who would then have less incentive to indulge in a practice which otherwise would lead them to hysteria, epilepsy and convulsive attacks: the operation was still being advocated in the United States as late as 1894. Back in 1786 S G Vogel had suggested that infibulations – preventing full erection by fastening the foreskin to the penis with silver wire – might be an effective preventative: comparable methods might be employed on girls. In 1864 the great French physician Broca told how he had performed an operation on a five-year old girl who had been masturbating repeatedly, despite surveillance and even the wearing of a chastity belt. By joining the labia, leaving only a small hole to pee through but covering the clitoris, he reduced the girl’s access to her sensitive parts. He was, however, criticised – though not for performing the operation, rather as to whether this was the most effective method. A colleague wondered why he had not tried cauterization, which he himself had used effectively on a boy, making his genital area so painful as to effectively discourage masturbation (Laqueur, 2002).
In 1891, London surgeon James Hutchinson proposed that male circumcision would effectively reduce the “shameful habit”, while his colleague, Spratling, went further, recommending “the complete section of the dorsal nerves of the penis”, while for females, “nothing short of ovariotomy” was advocated. Alas, even surgery was not certain of success. A Dr Richet in 1864 told how he had performed a total amputation of the clitoris of a 27-year old woman, but within a year she had learnt to obtain relief by masturbating her vagina. A Dr Guérin concurred, describing his own attempt to cure a patient, “I totally destroyed the clitoris without managing to extinguish the desire to masturbate”. But in 1894 Dr Eyer, of St John’s Hospital in Ohio, reported greater success when his patient, a little girl whose clitoris he had hacked away, reported “You know there is nothing there now, so I could do nothing” (cited in Ellis, 1924).
Reasoning in circles
These surgeons describe their procedures in clinical terms, much as they would the removal of an appendix or any other operation. They make no reference to the psychological trauma involved, not only subjecting the young patients to terrifying surgery but involving them in a horrific experience of induced guilt. Dr Yellowlees, in England, told in 1876 how he had been struck by “the conscious-stricken way in which they submitted to the operation on their penises” (Laqueur, 2002). The child was made to feel an outcast, a sinner, performing an unnatural act, which was vile in the eyes of God and of all decent men and women. The result was that children were terrorised, and in extreme cases this could lead to suicide; in uncounted instances, it led to feelings of guilt and self-recrimination which recur not only in case histories but also the literature of the period, ranging from Dean Farrar’s story of school life, Eric or little by little, to Tolstoy’s The Kreutzer sonata. Yet, while some doctors deplored the psychological effects of inculcating guilt and remorse, others welcomed it as a means whereby the patient himself would be driven to voluntarily abandon the practice. French doctor Debreyne in 1844 declared that masturbators “must be threatened with dishonour, with ignominy, with all the horrors of the most painful, the most degrading and the most shameful maladies and finally with an early death to be followed by eternal punishment” (Laqueur, 2002). A colleague, Dr Devay, taught his young patients that their penis would become gangrenous as a result. Privately he admitted that there was no truth in this whatever, but insisted that the lie was justified if it served his purpose.
These examples illustrate the extraordinary circular arguments of the delusion. Doctors justified themselves in threatening consequences they knew to be false, in order to discourage practices which they believed to be harmful in other ways. They demonstrate that, while it was unquestionably the ostensibly scientific aspect of the delusion which enabled it to take such universal hold, it would hardly have done so without a strong moral basis, explicit or concealed. At this time, when almost everyone in advanced societies still, if only nominally, subscribed to Judeo-Christian beliefs, medical authorities did not hesitate to interweave science and religion. Thus Mary R Melendy, an American doctor and author of popular guides to sexual behaviour, wrote to mothers: “Go teach your boy… about these organs that make him specially a boy. Teach him that they are not impure, but… made by God for a definite purpose. Impress upon him that if these organs are… put to any use besides that for which God made them – and He did not intend they should be used at all until man is fully grown – they will bring disease and ruin upon those who abuse and disobey those laws which God has made to govern them” (cited in Ellis, 1924). Needless to say, she does not indicate where God promulgated any such laws, which would – if they existed – hardly be compatible with medical reality.
Indeed, the strength of the moral aspect of the delusion is illustrated by the comment of one of the first medical men to declare that masturbation is no more harmful than sexual intercourse, Sir James Paget. Even while insisting that the practice is not the monster it had been made out to be, he concludes by saying, almost reluctantly, “I wish that I could say something worse of so nasty a practice: an uncleanliness, a filthiness forbidden by God, an unmanliness despised by man” (cited in Szasz, 1971).
Because masturbation is by definition generally a solitary act, and because given the climate of opinion most people kept quiet about their activities, there are no figures to show how prevalent the practice was or how many children were affected. When in 1949 Alfred Kinsey, professor of zoology, and his colleagues at Indiana University, presented their report on the sexual habits of the American male (followed in 1953 by a sequel on the female) they disclosed that 93% of American males, and 62% of females, indulged in masturbation, despite the fact that the taboo was still very widely imposed (Ellis, 1924). Since there is no reason to think that human nature had changed dramatically in the previous 100 years, we can suppose that even at the height of the delusion, and in the face of such a barrage of denunciation, almost every child felt the urge to masturbate, and that a very high percentage yielded to that urge. That the practice was widespread was generally admitted, but authorities, with rare exception, did not ask the obvious question: why, in that case, were the numbers of mad people relatively few?
Sceptical voices
Although, throughout the duration of the delusion, a few sceptical voices had been raised, this was only in medical circles and not in public. It was not until the very close of the 19th century that ideas began to change, and even then it was only slowly and reluctantly. A prominent influence was the English sociologist Havelock Ellis, whose writings were initially banned, but whose authority went a considerable way towards shaking the entrenched belief. Yet though in 1900 he was able to report that “recent authorities are almost unanimous in rejecting masturbation as a cause of insanity” (Szasz, 1971) the delusion was slow to die. Freud and his followers, even if they no longer held extreme views, did not question that masturbation was harmful (the importance Freud attributed to the subject is indicated by the fact that there are more than 100 references to masturbation in his collected works, as well as some fifty to auto-eroticism). Even though medical authorities generally came to concede that masturbation had few if any harmful effects, Kinsey, writing about male masturbation in 1949, noted that the consequences of the delusion were still much in evidence half a century later: “It must be realized that masturbation is taboo and even strongly condemned among certain groups; and while college men more often admit their experience, there are males in some other groups who would admit almost any other kind of sexual activity before they would give a record of masturbatory experiences… Millions of boys have lived in continual mental conflict over this problem. For that matter, many a boy still does. Many boys pass through a periodic succession of attempts to stop the habit, inevitable failures in those attempts, consequent periods of remorse, the making of new resolutions and a new start on the whole cycle. It is difficult to imagine anything better calculated to do permanent damage to the personality of an individual” (Szasz, 1971).
The situation was similar for the female. Four years later he reported:
In view of the more than two thousand years of religious condemnation of masturbation, fortified by the ostensibly scientific opinions of physicians and other professionally trained groups, it is not surprising that many individuals, both female and male, are considerably disturbed when they masturbate. Among the females in the sample who had ever masturbated, approximately half had experienced some psychological disturbance over their experience… This means that some millions of the females in the United States, and a larger number of the males, have had their selfassurance, their social efficiency, and sometimes their sexual adjustments in marriage needlessly damaged – not by their masturbations, but by the conflict between their practice and the moral codes.
“Masturbational insanity was real enough,” declares Dr Alex Comfort, but it was not the ignorant masses who were insane, rather “it was affecting the medical profession,” whom he stigmatises as ‘anxiety makers’ (Szasz, 1971). Thomas Szasz (1971) comments: “We may wonder how learned men and the public alike could believe such nonsense, flagrantly contradicted by observation easily made among both men and animals,” and attributes it to the fact that man “is more interested in preserving popular explanations, which tend to consolidate the group, than in making accurate observations which tend to divide it”.
Laqueur’s 2002 thesis analyses the concept of masturbation as a cultural phenomenon rooted in time. The delusion was, paradoxically, a child of the Age of Enlightenment, when the status of the individual in relation to society was in the course of transformation. Masturbation, so essentially an individual practice, was felt to be anti-social, so when Marten and Tissot offered grounds, however dubious, for demonising the practice, their views were enthusiastically embraced (Szasz, 1971).
Thus the masturbation delusion served a stabilising purpose: it allowed people to continue to accept the Judeo-Christian moral code and perpetuate it into a scientific age. Fundamental to the delusion was the puritanical credo that any form of self-indulgence is inherently blameworthy, and non-procreative sexual activity especially so. Consequently, when ostensible scientific support for demonising masturbation was offered, moralists and scientists welcomed it alike. They accepted the false reasoning, which led to the belief in masturbatory insanity because it justified their subconscious conviction that masturbation was wrong. The moral judgment not only preceded, but also continued to underlie any supposed scientific evaluation.
References
Comfort, A. (1967). The anxiety makers. London: Nelson.
Ellis, H. (1924). Studies in the psychology of sex (3rd ed). Philadelphia: F A Davis.
Hall, L. A. (2003). “It was affecting the medical profession”: the history of masturbatory insanity revisited. Paedagogica Historica, 39(6).
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C.E. (1949). Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W B Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: W B Saunders.
Laqueur, T. W. (2002). Solitary sex. New York: Zone Books.
Rengade, J. (1881). La vie normale. Paris: Libraire Illustrée.
Stall, S. (1909). What a young boy ought to know. Philadelphia: Environment Publishing.
Stengers, J., & Van Neck, A. (2002). Masturbation. Translated from the French Histoire d’une grande peur, la masturbation, by Kathryn Hoffmann. Originally published 1998. English translation: London: Palgrave.
Szasz, T. S. (1971). The manufacture of madness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
This article is adapted from an entry in the Encyclopedia of Exceptional Social Behaviour, which Robert Bartholomew and Hilary Evans have been working on together for a number of years.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 17, Issue 4, from 2006.
Mary Rodwell runs a UFO abductee/contactee support group in Perth, Western Australia, and produces associated print materials, videos, etc., aimed at promoting and endorsing extraterrestrial interpretations of the reported experiences and at supporting experiencers who embrace this kind of interpretation. Her book Awakening: How Extraterrestrial Contact Can Transform Your Life (Rodwell, 2002) represents a recent major piece of ‘Rodwelliana’.
Rodwell is, of course, entitled to her own view of how such reports are to be interpreted and of how these matters should be handled. But the nature of the book hinders critical assessment of her claims: it has a popular and often an emotional tone which militates against scepticism or even neutral scientific analysis and discourages the consideration of alternative hypotheses. Some of her procedures simply exclude such views, and she certainly does not treat the standard sceptical points at all fairly. In fact, Rodwell seems to think that scepticism insults the reporters by treating them as unreliable or even of unsound mind, whereas she herself believes that they often have advanced psychological/psychic abilities. (Of course, this does not follow: there is a difference between taking people seriously and accepting their stories as literally true.)
In contrast, Rodwell accepts without debate many alleged phenomena which are heavily disputed for want of persuasive evidence and in some cases are rejected by almost all the relevant scholars, such as the reality of ‘ancient astronauts’, UFO-related implants, ‘missing pregnancies’, psychic phenomena and powers, etc., etc. And she often provides little or no solid evidence for her own (dramatic) claims; nor does she provide references. Scientists will gather that no careful treatment is to be expected from her, and it can hardly be seen as surprising or disreputable if they ignore her material. Linguistic issues are potentially important in this area and my own introduction to Rodwell’s work involved her video on the subject. The treatment of language in this present book is rather more limited, but some comments are in order.
Firstly, some claims made (repeated from other sources) are so dramatic that very strong evidence is required if they are to be accepted. One excellent example of this involves Leir’s claims regarding the advanced linguistic abilities of some human infants identified as ‘Star Children’. Some of these claims would, if true, revolutionise the study of child language acquisition; the most dramatic of all is the claim that some babies are able to read. But I know of no properly conducted experiments which would demonstrate or even suggest that such things occur, nor of any child language acquisition experts who take these claims at all seriously.
Secondly, forms presented as spoken and written alien language are discussed in the (largely self-reported) case studies, notably that of Tracey Taylor, who also appears prominently on Rodwell’s video. Taylor includes this material in an account of her lifelong pattern of experiences. Much of the discussion is subjective in tone, involving Taylor’s ‘feelings’ about the meanings of her experiences and her artistic and (quasi-) linguistic responses to them. The material is generated by means of automatic writing, however this may be interpreted, and Taylor links this process with an intuitively
and experientially derived ‘theory’ of the nature of the aliens whom she regards as responsible.
Unfortunately, few of the linguistic comments made here are specific enough to permit proper analysis or testing. The written material produced by Taylor and another contactee and provided here in plates (more is seen on the video) is described as ‘hieroglyphic’, although it is not clear what Taylor thinks this term means generally, or what it is supposed to mean in this context. It has the appearance of text written ‘grassstroke’ style in a range of large alphabets, syllabaries, or (parts of) logographies. There is too little material in each sample to be more confident, especially in the absence of useful translations.
Taylor is reported as being able to write in more than one ‘unusual’ script (presumably in otherwise unknown languages; but few non-linguists make this distinction clearly). She can also reportedly speak in several ‘strange’ languages and ascribe meaning to some of this material and to her experience-inspired artwork. She adds that she and other experiencers regularly acquire such languages and, in due course, the ability to translate them into human languages without conscious learning. Unfortunately, evidence that these claims hold up and that these languages are genuine is not presented here, which is a huge omission given the very dramatic nature of the claims.
The corroboration reported by Taylor from other members of her groups is too vague and too informal to be taken seriously. For example, the comments about ‘ancient symbols’ found in temples and pyramids and about similarities between Taylor’s material and ‘hieroglyphic text’ are far too vague to be of use, and it is not at all clear that the people who were commenting had any intellectual authority in this area.
The samples of Taylor’s spoken material on Rodwell’s video appear to resemble glossolalia (‘speaking in tongues’), in which case the material is probably merely phonetic rather than linguistic and thus is not meaningful (though such phenomena are still very interesting in themselves). It is striking in this context that some of the sequences are reminiscent of Japanese, a language to which Taylor has been exposed. (I identified this as a possibility before learning that Taylor had lived in Japan.) It is characteristic of glossolalia and the like that the vast majority of the sounds produced are drawn from languages known or familiar to the speaker. A further reason for supposing that this present case involves glossolalia or a similar phenomenon rather than a genuine alien language derives from the fact that all the sounds used are familiar from human languages – and indeed not even confined to obscure languages unlikely to be known to speakers or their acquaintances. Genuine non-human (and non-terrestrial) languages would be expected to manifest different phonetic ranges.
If useful translations (preferably morpheme-by-morpheme) were provided for any of this material (spoken or written), it is possible that this kind of negative judgment might be proved mistaken. In this case, the material might be deemed genuinely linguistic and the issue would then be whether the language was indeed from an alien source, as claimed, or was of human invention. However, as will be seen, this sort of evidence appears unlikely to be produced.
In her summary, Rodwell herself raises some of these partly linguistic issues at a more general level. She reports that various viewers of her video have stated that they recognise some of the symbolism and linguistic material, suggesting a commonality of experience transcending the often very different locales and specific events involved. Some of these viewers also state that they find speaking the alien languages which they have acquired very enjoyable and indeed more natural than speaking their own first languages. Given these very strong claims, it is again unfortunate that no better evidence for them has yet been seen.
Another passage is, however, even more unfortunate in its implications. Rodwell quotes Taylor as making a number of highly obscure and/or implausible claims about the alien languages and as drawing further conclusions from these points which are inevitably contentious in the extreme. Then, in the middle of this passage, Taylor makes a more readily interpretable claim which is apparently associated with some of the above claims and which has very dramatic upshots. She states that in these alien languages “there is no preconceived idea or concept about what a particular sound actually means because this type of language is not structured in the way the English language is”. One assumes that she means here to contrast the alleged alien languages with all human languages rather than with English specifically, because the gist of this claim is that these languages cannot be analysed as human languages can. The claim is already incoherent, because individual sounds (as noted above) are themselves meaningless in human languages too; as expressed here, the contrast is
thus invalid. But the importance ascribed by Taylor to this point suggests that she means by the term sounds to refer to morphemes; her next comment, indeed, is “a particular sound or word is not related to a particular description or meaning” (my italics). Taylor then indicates (in her own words) that this means (as indeed it surely would mean) that the meaning of each utterance could not be related to that of earlier utterances and would have to be (somehow) arrived at intuitively (?) and presumably ‘holistically’ on each occasion.
The most damaging aspect of this passage is that it is implied (and indeed this is further hinted at by Rodwell herself) that analysis of these alien languages – no matter how sophisticated and free of advance assumptions based on the nature of human languages – is most unlikely to succeed. Such analysis would be more or less impossible, because morphemes with constant meanings could not be identified, and larger morphological and syntactic structures with more complex meanings could not be analysed as composed of these morphemes in significant specific orders and relationships (linear or other). (This is the normal practice in analysing previously unanalysed human languages or – suitably modified – other communication systems.)
However, it appears unlikely in the extreme that all this could be true. Any system which is recognisable as a language in the first place must thereby (by definition) have a complex and largely stable and well-defined structure of this kind (in general terms). That is the kind of thing that a language is. Languages (and indeed virtually all communication systems) depend upon the repetition of meaningful units. No ‘holistic’ interpretations unrelated to earlier texts are possible (although sometimes naive non-linguists using their first languages may perhaps have the subjective impression that this is happening). It is difficult to see how even a genuinely alien language could differ in such a fundamental respect and still be usable for its native speakers or for anyone else. Members of another species which really had the psychological abilities which this implies (assuming that these are possible in principle!) would presumably not need or use language. And it is not clear how they could succeed (or why they would expect to succeed) in using systems of this kind to communicate with humans, given our own psychological and linguistic capabilities and habits.
It is true that even human languages vary a great deal in structural terms, and a genuinely alien language might well be very much more differently structured, perhaps in some relatively fundamental ways in respect of which human languages do not differ. Analysis of such radically novel systems might be very difficult and error-prone (especially with access only to human learners, not to native users). But this would not necessarily be an impossible task in principle. The point that humans who are naïve non-linguists can allegedly learn and use such languages would itself suggest that the differences would not be as great as might be logically possible or even probable – or as great as Taylor and Rodwell suggest in denying that the languages are morphologically structured. In this context one should note that (as stated) the phonetics, which can be observed directly and thus described readily without any comprehension, are unremarkable.
However, it is also true that any ‘system’ which was presented as a language but which in fact really did have no largely stable and well-defined structure could not be analysed (or at least could not be analysed using any techniques currently known). In such a case, no quasilinguistic claims made about this ‘language’ (e.g. about the meanings of sequences in it) could be empirically tested, and all such claims would be immune from scientific scrutiny (unless and until wholly new principles of analysis could be developed; but this would appear unlikely to occur).
One cannot be blamed for suspecting that claims of this kind might have been developed with the aim of preventing scientific analysis of this material and thus blocking any possible demonstration that the nature of the material was (or might very well be) not as described (non-linguistic, concocted, etc). This would certainly be the actual effect of adopting such a position; nothing useful could be said about such material (other than about the phonetics).
However, once again, the onus is, in fact, upon those making these dramatic claims to justify them or at least to co-operate in rendering them testable. If the systems identified as alien languages are such that the associated claims can be tested, they should be so presented. If the claims are really untestable, their advocates must realise that these systems will be of limited interest to linguists and other scientists, and that these scholars are liable to adopt (legitimately) the default interpretation that the alien languages are not genuine. In order to determine the real situation, we must obtain a reasonably sized corpus of data in each such language and be allowed to work with those who claim ability in it, so as to determine its actual structure.
Rodwell does refer to the critical work of Gary Anthony and his associates (one of whom is myself) on
the linguistic aspects of her case. Her comments are perfunctory and somewhat loosely phrased. She seems inclined to fluctuate between what may be an over-optimistic expectation that work of this kind will ‘validate’ her claims and a defensive stance grounded in the evasive-sounding claims mentioned above. A reader might not obtain a clear idea of the real nature and force of this critical work from Rodwell’s own statements. I therefore repeat here that, as we stated in our article in the MUFON Journal (Anthony & Newbrook, 2002), we are very willing to examine any alleged language of this kind with open minds – but with suitable rigour.
In summary, Rodwell and her supporters will need to provide much better evidence – including evidence arising from such analysis as Anthony and I might conduct – before the balance of probability renders her case sufficiently interesting to warrant further focused attention. Like many writers in this area, they do not seem to realise that apparently outrageous claims such as these are simply not going to be accepted without very specific, very strong evidence. If I see such evidence,
my interest will be re-kindled.
References
Anthony, G., & Newbrook, M. (2002). Alien communication: An intriguing puzzle. MUFON Journal, 411
(July), 3–6.
Rodwell, M. (2002). Awakening: How Extraterrestrial Contact Can Transform Your Life. Leeds: Fortune Books. 18
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 17, Issue 4, from 2006.
Sigmund Freud’s work on religion, a subject he seemed immensely fascinated with despite his distaste for it as a modern-day practice, has not fared well under close scrutiny. Here I offer a critical assessment of Freud’s ideas about religion, before concluding to what extent they have anything useful to say about religion in the 21st century.
Despite Freud’s early medical specialization in neuropathology, it was his later interest in neuroses – their causes and treatment – and his investigation of the unconscious through psychoanalysis that established his reputation.
The basic goal of psychoanalysis is to make the unconscious conscious. This is because Freud believed that the roots of all psychological problems were hidden within the unconscious. Once the unconscious has become conscious, or accessible to the patient, then the cause of his/her problem becomes apparent, enabling the patient to deal with it in a conscious and rational manner. Freud believed that by getting patients to talk freely and without inhibition about their problems, under the guidance of the analyst, they would have insight into their unconscious ideas and motives, enabling them to tackle the previously hidden causes of their problems.
In Freud’s view personality is a dynamic system that develops during childhood. During this time assorted goals must be reached and conflicts must be resolved, e.g. the Oedipus complex. This relates to Freud’s suggestion that a male child has an unconscious jealousy and fear of his father and a desire for his mother; this creates a conflict for the child, which must be resolved, not externally, but internally. These conflicts, and the consequences that Freud believed resulted from a failure to resolve them, are central to Freud’s position on religion.
It may seem difficult to imagine how psychoanalytic theory could have any applications for religion, particularly as Freud, though Jewish by birth and upbringing, did not have any religious faith. Although Freud could not be said to be at all ashamed of his Jewish background, he was in no sense a religious man.
However, although he was neither interested in nor needful of religion for his own sake, he was intensely interested in why other people should need it and why it should have played such a predominant role in human history. He thought he was beginning to find the answers to these questions from evidence provided by his neurotic patients. These patients had failed to resolve successfully their childhood conflicts leading to them being driven below the level of consciousness, into the unconscious. Although temporarily buried there they had appeared in later life, in the form of irrational, ‘neurotic’ symptoms.
Neuroticism and Religion
Freud saw a resemblance between the behaviour of his neurotic patients and what he believed was the behaviour of religious people. He observed religion as an outsider and used Austrian Roman Catholicism as a model for all religious practice. From this extremely limited approach to the study of religion he came to understand it largely as a matter of the believer performing certain specific practices, observances, ceremonies and rites. Freud believed that this behaviour was similar to the private ceremonials that several of his patients obsessively indulged in, and that the similarities between the two types of behaviour were too great to be accounted for by mere coincidence.
Following this line of reasoning, Freud found several similarities between the neurotic and the religious person, e.g. a guilty conscience resulting from any omission of the neurotic or religious person’s ceremonial; the treatment of the ceremonial as something isolated from everything else in life; necessary performance of acts of penance to maintain the subject’s peace of mind; and what Freud called the ‘mechanism of physical displacement’, by which he meant a mental value being given to the ceremonial out of all proportion to its intrinsic importance.
From his identification of these similarities Freud came to the conclusion that religion was best understood as a neurosis, and because of its place in the history of mankind, he called it a “universal obsessional neurosis” (Freud, 1962, p. 39).
Freud believed that religion could be treated and cured in the same way as any other neurosis: if the religious practitioner became aware of why s/he was indulging in neurotic behaviour s/he would be able to face reality without the need for that behaviour.
Totem, Taboo and Oedipus Rex
Freud was also interested in the reasons for the origin and persistence of religion as a universal obsessional neurosis. He looked for answers in primitive society and these ‘answers’ formed the basis for his book, Totem and Taboo. In it Freud argues that all primitive societies went through a stage of totemism. A totem is a symbolic emblem of a particular social group within a tribe. An object of reverence or worship, it is protected by taboos which generally forbid killing it, eating it, or even touching it.
Freud interpreted the totem as representing the father, because he knew of three cases in which boys with Oedipal conflicts had phobias of animals in which the animal seemed to be a substitute for the father. Freud believed that the fundamental taboos of totemism correspond to the two repressed wishes of the Oedipus complex.
These conclusions were based upon the ‘primal horde’ theory, originally proposed by Darwin and Robertson Smith, but taken up by Freud. The theory proposes a period of human prehistory in which the family unit consisted of father, mother, and offspring. The father, as the dominant male, retained the exclusive rights to the females and drove away or killed any of the sons who challenged his position.
The sons couldn’t defeat the father-leader individually so they banded together to kill him and, being cannibals, ate him. Hick (1973, p. 34) summarizes what Freud believed resulted from this unsavoury act:
This was the primal crime, the patricide that has set up tensions within the human psyche out of which have developed moral inhibitions, totemism, and the other phenomena of religion. For having slain their father, the brothers are struck with remorse. They also find that they cannot all succeed to his position and there is a continuing need for restraint. The dead father’s prohibition accordingly takes on a new (‘moral’) authority as a taboo against incest. This association of religion with the Oedipus complex, which is renewed in each male individual, is held to account for the mysterious authority of God in the human mind and the powerful guilt feelings which make people submit to such a fantasy.
Freud is claiming that the origins of religion and morality can be traced back to an actual historical event. Or to put it in Freud’s terms, religion is a ‘return of the repressed’. For Freud, God is, in every case, modelled after either the father or the need for a ‘father image’ (rising from the Oedipus complex), and an individual’s personal relationship with God is dependent upon their relationship with their physical father.
According to Freud, all religions are attempts to deal with the sense of guilt deriving from the primal crime. For example, Freud’s interpretation of Christianity would be that Christ took his own life to make amends for his brothers’ primal crime and at the same time took revenge on the father, on his brothers’ behalf, by becoming a god in place of the father.
Having diagnosed religion as a ‘universal obsessional neurosis’ and having accounted for its origins, he summed up his position, that religion is an illusion with no future, in his book, The Future of an Illusion. Religion is, Freud explained, merely humankind’s psychological defence against the forces of nature. Just as children find relief from the terrors of nature in the love of their parents, adults also feel terrified and helpless when facing the universe, and so seek protection from an all-powerful father figure, one who is capable of controlling nature, enforcing moral rules, and easing the fear of death.
When men and women through the ages thought that their worship and theology was responding to a reality other than themselves they were in actual fact simply using psychological defence mechanisms. Freud believed that if people could use psychoanalysis to enable them to understand that they were using these defence mechanisms then they would soon be equipped to face the cruel reality of the world without the need for the fraudulent aid of religion.
Validity, Falsifiability and Global Religions
There are some worthwhile elements to Freud’s interpretation of religion. It offers an explanation as to how it arose, what is going on when people believe in God, and it points the way forward to enable humans to stand on their own feet without the illusion which is, in Freud’s eyes, religion. It also seems to have some scientific backing as it is allegedly based on the observation of infant behaviour, of adult patients’ neurotic behaviour and recovery, and on the beliefs and practices of primitive tribes. Finally, some elements of his interpretation have a degree of plausibility to them. However, we can also criticise his position on religion on several grounds.
The validity of Freud’s procedures has been questioned and his theories are considered to be unfalsifiable, based on flawed research, and to have limited scientific objectivity. For example, his speculations about a primal horde, and indeed much of what he wrote about totem and taboo, appears to have little scientific basis. From anthropological studies there is no evidence that
a primal horde dominated by a single male ever existed, and Darwin derived his notion from hearsay reports about the organization of gorilla troops that have since been shown to be false. In addition, in 1943, following the Survey of Objective Studies of Psychoanalytic Concepts, Sears (cited by Clark, 1958, p. 88) reported that empirical studies are far from verifying all of Freud’s hypotheses, including those concerning parent-child relations (Freud emphasised the importance of the child’s relations with his/her parents for future religious development). Sixty years on, many of Freud’s hypotheses have yet to be verified.
Another problem with Freud’s view of religion is that in logical terms it commits what Shaw (1978) calls a “genetic fallacy”. Freud thought that he had discovered the psychological origin for belief and concluded from this that the belief must be illusory. This is an entirely illegitimate conclusion because the motive for a belief doesn’t affect its truth or falsehood.
Freud’s view of religion is exclusively paternal in basis. The importance of female goddesses is passed over entirely and Freud neglects any discussion of the possible importance of the mother in totemic religion. These omissions are characteristic of psychoanalytic theory, which until more recently habitually emphasized the father’s role at the expense of the mother’s.
Another critical point is that if religious behaviour is best understood as neurotic behaviour, we would expect its most serious and intense practitioners to exhibit the most severe neurotic symptoms. Although it is extremely difficult to judge the mental health of historical figures it is worth noting that most (though certainly not all) models of religious life throughout history have not been recorded as individuals crippled by disturbed behaviour. Instead they are seen to embody the qualities of selflessness and spiritual growth.
Finally, there appears to be some dissimilarity between the God that Freud describes and the God worshipped by Christians. For Freud, God’s main function seems to be as a father figure who offers protection both from punishment and from the harsh forces of nature, in return for loyalty and obedience. From Freud’s external view of Christianity, this may be how Christians appeared to talk about and worship God (in Wittgenstein’s terminology we might describe Freud as being outside the Christian language game). However, most Christians would find it difficult if not impossible to recognise their God from Freud’s description.
The description of ‘God as father’ shares some similarity with the definition of a human father, but it is not intended to reflect primarily his power and willingness to guarantee worshippers protection from natural disasters, the effects of guilt or from extinction, as Freud suggests; rather it is an attempt to describe a reciprocal relationship with him, defined more in terms of love than of protective power. This relationship, real or not, does not suggest a retreat into an illusory place of refuge.
In Freud’s favour, we can see his ideas not as a generalisation for all religion and religious people but as an explanation for the views of certain individuals. These people can be seen as having a false religious belief, one that they hold purely for the sense of security it provides. Psychoanalysis may actually help these people to realise that their belief is an illusory one, based more on internal conflict than faith. It is not the job of psychoanalysis to tell a person whether to believe in God or not, but instead to provide an insight into their concept of God – to identify whether it is a healthy one for them.
Similarly, Freud argued that all religious people use defence mechanisms, such as denial and rationalization. While this may be an exaggeration, it isn’t unreasonable to suggest that most believers use these defence mechanisms to some extent as part of their religious belief, but perhaps no more than anyone else.
Freud’s view of religion involved exaggeration and overestimation of its negative causes and consequences. This was no doubt driven by his own atheism and his lack of direct contact with modern religion. However, this does not diminish any insights that psychoanalysis might bring to individuals, whether they are religious or not, and whether the relationships they are trying to improve are with people or their gods.
References
Clark, W. H. (1958). The Psychology of Religion. Toronto: Macmillan.
Freud, S. (1983). Totem and Taboo. London: Ark Paperbacks.
Freud, S. (1962). The Future of an Illusion (Revised ed.). London: Hogarth.
Hick, J. (1973). Philosophy of Religion (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Shaw, D. (1978). The Dissuaders, Three Explanations of Religion. London: SCM Press.
This article originally appeared in The Skeptic, Volume 17, Issue 4, from 2006.
What I want to do is explain how people can come to believe in extremely traumatic events that never happened. It is quite clear that this does happen in the case of alien abductees, or in the cases of medical virgins who remembered being raped during their childhood. It is also quite clear to me from my research that this has happened in thousands, if not millions, of cases in North America and the UK, but I cannot prove that assertion. Still, some cases of illusory memories are provable.
I am going to cover seven major points:
motivation
secondary gain
belief systems
authority figures
use of hypnosis, dream analysis, body symptoms and other kinds of theories
rehearsal (of imagined ‘memories’)
cognitive dissonance.
Motivation
Firstly, to get someone to remember something horrible that never happened to them in their childhood, they have to be very motivated. There is a common misconception that therapists can ‘implant’ memories. I do not like the word implant at all.
In order to believe in repressed memories, you have to be very motivated, and your motivation usually involves a quest to solve the puzzle of your life. We all want to have explanations for what has happened to us, and we all tend to seek fairly simple explanations, so it is very appealing to say, “Well, I have trouble with relationships, I have an eating disorder, I have trouble with my self-esteem, and these are symptoms of sexual abuse, so maybe I was abused and repressed the memory”.
During the height of the recovered memory movement, in the late 1980s and early 1990s, this was a common belief system, and many still believe it. So, many people – particularly women seeking therapy – were highly motivated to come up with a solution to their life problems.
Secondary gain
Secondly, there was secondary gain involved in almost every case I investigated. What I mean by that is that people, by being victims of sexual abuse, got a lot of attention they would not ordinarily have had. They got a lot of sympathy and, not to be harsh about it, they also could avoid a lot of responsibility for things in their lives at various points. This is not to say that this kind of belief system did not also cause extreme suffering, but there is no question that there was secondary gain.
Belief systems
The third point is, you have to have a belief in the theory of massive repression or massive dissociation, and many people did, and many people still do. When I was living in England for two months in the spring of 2003, I did an informal survey of people as I was travelling on trains, or when I was in pubs, or when I was walking up and down the barge canal. I asked people, “If you were eight years old and you had a terrible, terrible thing happen to you, do you think that you could block that completely out, not have any memory of it, and then remember it later in life?”
The vast majority of people said, “Oh yes, you can do that, that happens.” I then asked, “Well, how do you know?”, and they would answer, “Well, I just know,” or “I’ve seen it on television or in a movie”. So – you have to have a belief that massive repression is something that people can do.
Authority figures
It also helps, although it is not necessary, to have an authority figure you go to who says, “Oh yes, that’s true. I know this is true because I have a PhD [or “I’m a psychiatrist” or “I’m a social worker”], and I’ve seen many people come through my office who had exactly the same symptoms that you do – these troubled relationships, problems with self-esteem and eating disorders – and many of them had these memories come back, that they had not remembered for many years, of being sexually abused, and so I think you may have repressed memories, too.”
It really is an encouragement to illusory memories, but it is not necessary. I want to emphasise that. You don’t need an authority figure – illusory memories can be totally self-induced and, in many cases, they are.
Use of hypnosis, dream analysis, body symptoms, and other kinds of theories
A great deal has been said or written about the hazards of hypnosis, but I do want to add other things, and this is primarily what you will find in the chapter on “How to Believe the Unbelievable” in my book, Victims of Memory (Pendergrast, 1996). In this chapter, I went through tick, tick, tick; these are the ways that you can come to believe things that did not happen. Certainly hypnosis (or guided imagery, visualisation, meditation, or prayer, which are all forms of hypnosis or auto-hypnosis if used to try to recall ‘repressed’ memories) is a very good way to do that, particularly if the authority figure who is leading you in the form of hypnosis has a vested interest in this theory of massive repression and believes that you may very well have been abused.
I also want to warn about something called ‘inadvertent cueing’. Many therapists are told, “Don’t use leading questions with people, don’t lead your clients”. I do not think that anybody does intentionally lead their clients, but I interviewed many, many therapists who believed in repressed memories, and they all did lead their clients. They told me in the next sentence, after they had told me exactly how they had led their clients – “but you must be very careful never to lead your clients. I always maintain a totally neutral stance”, and so on.
If you believe in this idea that you can forget years of horrible things and then remember them much later, you are likely to convey that belief to your clients. And so I have told therapists – “Be careful what you believe!” I think that ultimately this whole thing comes down to a belief system. Again, I just want to emphasise that.
It does not really matter what modalities you use. As Harvard Professor of Psychology Richard McNally found in his studies, many people have a very firm belief that they are incest survivors without having any actual memory of anything happening to them. They simply believe it, and once you believe it, I think that it is almost a foregone conclusion that you will come up with something.
For instance, recovered memory therapists use dream analysis. Frequently we dream about things that we are worried about, and if you are in therapy and you think that maybe your father sexually abused you, or that someone else did, you begin to obsess over it, and that is precisely what you will dream about. Consequently, many of these things become self-fulfilling prophecies. The same thing is true of so-called body memories where they tell you that you may have some panic attack or you may have some bodily symptom and then you sort of work yourself up into it, or pay particular attention to it.
Rehearsal
Once you come up with a scenario – and I saw this over and over again in this type of misguided therapy – you come up with a fragmentary image. What would happen would be that the therapists would take these fragmentary images and then they would have people rehearse them over and over again. In fact, they would tell them, “Pretend that you have a movie screen or a television screen in your head and you have to visualise it, and you have to zoom in and freeze frame”. They would literally tell people to do those things – it was all very visual. So people would develop a script, a narrative, and they would have them write the narrative down over and over again, or repeat it in group sessions over and over again.
The more you repeat something and the more you rehearse it, the more it becomes true for you. Many retractors who took back their memory beliefs because they decided that they were incorrect, still cannot get rid of the intrusive images. They have post-traumatic stress disorder. That would be an interesting thing to study – these people who have gone through this kind of therapy and developed a false belief system, then disbelieved it, but they still cannot get rid of the intrusive memories of something they know rationally did not happen to them, such as, say, being in a satanic cult.
Cognitive dissonance
This is a theory that was put forward by Leon Festinger quite a few years ago. It is quite an interesting theory and I think it makes sense, but it is just a theory. The idea is that you cannot have two contradictory ideas in your head at the same time. One of them is going to push the other one out like a cuckoo pushing an egg out from the nest. So if you opt for the idea that daddy did this horrible thing to you, you cannot very well have the idea that daddy was also a loving parent who did all these nice things with you, even though you have these very valid memories that he was a nice guy in many ways. So once you plump down on the side of this new belief system it is almost like a see-saw that goes ‘whomp’, and to ‘unwhomp’ it is very, very difficult to do. Once somebody opts for a belief system, and invests in it, and goes public with it, it is extremely difficult to undo.
Many, many times, people have said to me, “Why would anybody make up something so dreadful? Why would anybody want to make up something so horrible about someone as central in their lives as their parents?” But it is not a matter of wanting – it is a matter of having a seed planted in your mind and having it grow almost inevitably. So it is really a belief system, followed by methods that really are quite suggestive to your memory. Memories are always reconstructive, and they can be changed – sometimes permanently. So I can only hope that you can remember some of what I have written here at least fairly accurately.
Reference
Pendergrast, M. (1996). Victims of Memory: Incest Accusations and Shattered Lives (Revised British edition). London: HarperCollins.