Home Blog Page 111

Ipso Factoid: Milking it?

0

The Daily Mail has reported on the stress busting properties of milk.  It seems a protein in milk known as Lactium could be the answer.  The protein they are actually talking about is called casein (also mentioned in the article as a stress relieving protein), whereas Lactium is the compound that has been created from casein by French company Ingredia, who are promoting it as a way to “manage chronic daily stress as well as occasional stress”, apparently with no toxic side effects.  This sounds pretty good.

Apparently there is scientific evidence in animals and in humans demonstrating the effects of Lactium in stress.  According to the article Lactium has only been tested on 190 volunteers but one theory of how it works is that it “acts on receptors in the brain that help control anxiety and stress”. It is not mentioned which receptors or why this might be the case.  A search of pubmed quickly reveals, well, very little.  There are no articles about Lactium or casein in relation to use in combating stress in humans.  From searching through Google it looks like the 190 volunteers who have been tested came from 5 studies that are not easy to get hold of.  One such paper by Dr Marta Santuré tested Lactium against placebo.  It is not clear which journal this study is published in, or if it is published at all.  It measured the effects of stress in 63 women who were suffering from at least 1 symptom of stress, measured by an unnamed questionnaire that is not described.  Somewhat unsurprisingly it has favourable results showing that in 5 out of 8 areas of stress (remember that the participants had to have at least 1 symptom, and since no further participant information is given, it is unclear how many participants fell into each group) Lactium was significantly better than placebo.  The researcher even suggests that the insignificant results such as in locomotive symptoms could have been significant if they hadn’t been masked by the troublesome placebo effect.  Interestingly, despite giving a ringing endorsement for Lactium, showing positive results at a dosage of 150mg/day the LactiumUSA website where this research is linked from claims that Lactium has been proven effective in humans at 167mg/day.

So how did this little known effect of Lactium find its way to the Daily Mail?  It may just be coincidence, but at the bottom of the article is the answer to the question of where to find this remarkable product in the UK: Boots.  They are selling “Equilibrium” containing Lactium at a price of £9.99 for a 14 day supply.  The boots website has this to say about it “This product contains Lactium a unique ingredient which is proven to help with the stresses of every day life, helping you through a stressful day” and “This one a day supplement contains the patented ingredient Lactium.  All Boots vitamins and suppliers are checked to ensure they meet our high quality and safety standards”.  Presumably Boots had more luck tracking down the other 4 studies showing safety and efficacy data.

B Premanand 1930 – 2009.

0

Basava Premanand was India’s leading skeptic and humanist; he published The Indian Skeptic and was a teacher, debunker,and performer.People compare him to James Randi, but it might actually be more appropriate to compare Randi to him.

I met him once, when Lewis Jones did an interview with him for The Skeptic. Seeing him was the same kind of mind-blowing experience that seeing Randi for the first time was. He *looked* like a guru; he performed the miracles that India’s gurus use to attract followers; and then he explained exactly what he’d done like Agatha Christie. Simply brilliant.

The thing that has always stuck in my mind from the interview was that he said that debunking miracles was very important in India because miracles are how religions/religious leaders sell themselves (with the implication that once they have sold themselves all kinds of exploitation become possible). This seems to me an important point to keep in mind when someone asks, “What harm can it do if people believe…?”

The Times did a pretty intelligent writeup of his work in 2003: http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/life_and_style/article1152564.ece

And the BBC here, in 2004: http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/programmes/this_world/3813469.stm

Premanand took a lot of risk in trenchantly opposing gurus he believed to be fraudulent. I hope among his many admierers are some who can carry on his work.

Wendy Grossman www.pelicancrossing.net

Night Terrors: Sleep paralysis

0

Nat Guest explores and explains the phenomenon of sleep paralysis; the state between sleep and wakefulness.

It rolled over her and landed bodily on her chest. There it sat. It breathed airlessly, pressing her, sapping her. ‘Oh, no. A Sitting Ghost.’
– Maxine Hong Kingston, The Woman Warrior

I was once attacked by a lion in my own bed.

At least, I thought it was a lion. All auditory and sensory input was pointing to it being a lion; the rasping, growling in my ear; its hot breath in my face; the enormous shaggy weight of it on top of me and the razor sharp teeth and claws mauling at my neck.

But not quite all input. Because how ever hard I tried, I could not open my eyes, and I could not move any part of my body. I could feel my bed beneath me, and I could feel the sheets around me. I could feel what position I was lying in. I simply could not move. I, like somewhere between 25-40% of the population (depending on which study you go by), was suffering from an episode of sleep paralysis.

Sleep paralysis is a phenomenon which can occur in the state between REM sleep, where dreaming occurs, and waking up. During REM sleep, the brain paralyses the body to avoid us carrying out our dream-actions and harming ourselves somehow. Sometimes, on waking, the brain does not quite turn off these dreams – or the paralysis accompanying them – resulting in a potentially intensely frightening experience. I have always regarded sleep paralysis as lucid dreaming’s ugly sister, in that both occur when there is some discrepancy between different parts of the brain, some parts of which still believe you are asleep and continue to dream away happily, whilst other parts are lucid and know full well that you are actually awake.

When the paralysis is accompanied by a feeling of a weight or malevolent presence crushing you, as mine was, it is known as ‘old hag’ syndrome. This ‘heaviness’ on your chest may be accompanied by other hallucinations, auditory, visual or tactile, and a feeling of terror and mounting panic. These hallucinations are known as hynagogic (if occurring at the onset of sleep) or hypnopompic (if occurring just before waking). The phenomenon of sleep paralysis is little-heard of by most, although its influence is far-reaching; echoes of it run throughout human mythology and folklore, amid superstitions that it was caused by witches (the ‘old hag’), demons and other evil spirits sitting on your chest. Indeed, the ‘-mare’ of ‘nightmare’ actually comes from the Anglo-Saxon word ‘merren’, meaning ‘to crush’, because of exactly these associations. Though this type of explanation may seem outdated now, it has by no means disappeared. A colleague of mine once recounted to me the story of how, waking in the middle of the night, her husband had felt a presence sitting on his chest and attempting to strangle him. Understandably scared, and having heard stories from neighbours about his house being haunted, her husband attributed the strangling to a malevolent spirit and had been living in fear ever since.

Another common manifestation of sleep paralysis is ‘the visitor’, where the sleeper will wake, helpless and unable to move (their eyes may be open or closed), and have a strong feeling that there is a something watching them from a corner of the room. It has been theorised that it is precisely this type of ‘visitation’ which leads to accounts of extra-terrestrial visits.

Though both of these hallucinatory types of experience can be terrifying, there are methods that can be employed to end the paralysis. Some people swear by concentrating on moving just one finger or toe, and say that once you have managed to move a tiny part of you, the spell will be broken. Some people try to scream or make a noise to in order wake themselves up. These techniques can be effective, but there is something to be said for simple awareness of the scientific explanation. On hearing of my colleague’s husband’s plight, I immediately printed off reams of information on the subject for him. She reported back that he was relieved to discover that what he had experienced was in fact something perfectly rational, and indeed relatively common.

After jolting fully awake from my own episode of paralysis to find that there was no lion or similar in my bedroom, it didn’t take long for me to realise what had actually happened; at which point it ceased immediately to be something horrific and monstrous and instead just led to a feeling of excitement. Though my experience of the event had not changed, the way I viewed it had. “Oh, well that was quite interesting”, I thought, “Can I do it again?”. This, perhaps, is the better way to view the darker, more mysterious workings of the mind: not with fear, but with curiosity.


Natalie Guest is a freelance writer and a member of today’s “lost generation”. She blogs sporadically and can play the spoons.

Why Statues Weep: The Best of the Skeptic

0

We are in the final stages of producing a “Best of the Skeptic” volume by the title of “Why Statues Weep”. The Skeptic magazine is the UK’s longest running publication devoted to examining science, secularism, psychology, critical thinking and claims of the paranormal. This collection covers a wide range of topics such as popular myths, UFOs, psychic fraudsters, claims of psychic healing and alien abduction, near-death experiences, false memories, and much, much more. The book includes contributions from Susan Blackmore, John Diamond, Edzard Ernst, Ray Hyman, Richard Wiseman, and many other leading sceptical commentators, as well as interviews with Stephen Fry, Paul Daniels, and Joe Nickell.

Advance orders are now be taken for delivery for Xmas 2009 at the special price of £9.99 within the UK and £11.99 elsewhere  (post free in both instances). The RRP for the book will be £11.99. Register with no commitment by calling 020 7841 1959 or by emailing [email protected]

Wendy M Grossman, Chris French & Simon Hoggart (eds.). (2009). Why Statues Weep: The Best of the Skeptic. London: Philosophy Press.

The perfect Xmas present?

Why We Need Science

0

Published on Science Based Medicine (18th August 2009)

Harriet Hall examines the reliability and explanatory power of anecdotal reports, and biases in human perception in her article Why We Need Science: “I saw it with my own eyes” Is Not Enough


I recently wrote an article for a community newspaper attempting to explain to scientifically naive readers why testimonial “evidence” is unreliable; unfortunately, they decided not to print it. I considered using it here, but I thought it was too elementary for this audience. I have changed my mind and I am offering it below (with apologies to the majority of our readers), because it seems a few of our readers still don’t “get” why we have to use rigorous science to evaluate claims. People can be fooled, folks. All people. That includes me and it includes you. Richard Feynman said

The first principle is that you must not fool yourself–and you are the easiest person to fool.

Science is the only way to correct for our errors of perception and of attribution. It is the only way to make sure we are not fooling ourselves. Either Science-Based Medicine has not done a good job of explaining these vital facts, or some of our readers are unable or unwilling to understand our explanations.

Our commenters still frequently offer testimonials about how some CAM method “really worked for me.” They fail to understand that they have no basis for claiming that it “worked.” All they can really claim is that they observed an improvement following the treatment. That could indicate a real effect or it could indicate an inaccurate observation or it could indicate a post hoc ergo propter hoc error, a false assumption that temporal correlation meant causation. Such observations are only a starting point: we need to do science to find out what the observations mean.

Last week one of our commenters wrote the worst testimonial yet:

I have witnessed first hand the life that begins to flow through the body upon the removal of a subluxation.

What does this even mean? Does he expect anyone to believe this just because he says it? Would he believe me if I said I had witnessed first hand the invisible dragon in Carl Sagan’s garage?

Another commenter wrote

Those pro SBM commenters here seem to think that even if they see something with their own eyes that they can’t believe it if there are no double blinded officially published studies to prove that what they saw actually happened.

Well, yes, that’s pretty much what we do think; and we are appalled that you don’t understand it yet, since it’s the whole reason we have to do science. I would phrase it a bit differently: “Seeing something with my own eyes doesn’t prove it’s true and it doesn’t preclude the necessity for scientific testing.”

We can’t believe our own eyes. The very process of vision itself is an interpretive construct by the brain. There are two blind spots in our field of vision and we aren’t even aware of them. I saw a magician cut a woman in half on stage – that was an illusion, a false perception. I saw a patient get better after a treatment – my interpretation that the treatment caused the improvement may have been a mistake, a false attribution.

So for those who still don’t get it, here’s my simplistic article:
—————————————

Sometimes We Get It Wrong

How can you know whether a medical treatment really works? If everybody says it works, and it worked for your Aunt Sally, and you try it and your symptoms go away, you can pretty well assume it really works. Right?

No, you can’t make that assumption, because sometimes we get it wrong. For many centuries doctors used leeches and lancets to relieve patients of their blood. They KNEW bloodletting worked. Everybody said it did. When you had a fever and the doctor bled you, you got better. Everyone knew of a friend or relative who had been at death’s door until bloodletting cured him. Doctors could recount thousands of successful cases.

All those people got it wrong. When George Washington got a bad throat infection, his doctors removed so much of his blood that his weakened body couldn’t recover, and he died. We finally got around to testing bloodletting and found out it did much more harm than good. Patients who got well had been getting well IN SPITE of bloodletting, not because of it. And some patients had died unnecessarily, like George Washington.

Even modern doctors sometimes get it wrong. Not long ago, doctors used to do an operation for heart disease where they opened the chest and tied off chest wall arteries to divert more blood flow to the heart. They had an impressive 90% success rate. A smart doctor named Leonard Cobb wanted to make sure, so he did an experiment where he just made the incision in the chest and closed it back up without actually doing anything. He discovered that just as many patients improved after the fake surgery! Doctors stopped doing that operation.

How could so many people be so wrong? How could they believe something had helped them when it actually had done them more harm than good? There are a whole lot of reasons people can come to believe an ineffective treatment works.

  1. The disease may have run its natural course. A lot of diseases are self-limiting; the body’s natural healing processes restore people to health after a time. A cold usually goes away in a week or so. To find out if a cold remedy works, you have to keep records of successes and failures for a large enough number of patients to find out if they really get well faster with the remedy than without it.
  2. Many diseases are cyclical. The symptoms of any disease fluctuate over time. We all know people with arthritis have bad days and good days. The pain gets worse for a while, then it gets better for a while. If you use a remedy when the pain is bad, it was probably about to start getting better anyway, so the remedy gets credit it doesn’t deserve.
  3. We are all suggestible. If we’re told something is going to hurt, it’s more likely to hurt. If we’re told something is going to make it better, it probably will. We all know this: that’s why we kiss our children’s scrapes and bruises. Anything that distracts us from thinking about our symptoms is likely to help. In scientific studies that compare a real treatment to a placebo sugar pill, an average of 35% of people say they feel better after taking the sugar pill. The real treatment has to do better than that if we’re going to believe it’s really effective.
  4. There may have been two treatments and the wrong one got the credit. If your doctor gave you a pill and you also took a home remedy, you may give the credit to the home remedy. Or maybe something else changed in your life at the same time that helped treat the illness and that was the real reason you got better.
  5. The original diagnosis or prognosis may have been incorrect. Lots of people have supposedly been cured of cancer who never actually had cancer. Doctors who tell a patient he only has 6 months to live are only guessing and can guess wrong. The best they can do is say the average patient with that condition lives 6 months – but average means half of people live longer than that.
  6. Temporary mood improvement can be confused with cure. If a practitioner makes you feel optimistic and hopeful, you may think you feel better when the disease is really unchanged.
  7. Psychological needs can affect our behavior and our perceptions. When someone wants to believe badly enough, he can convince himself he has been helped. People have been known to deny the facts – to refuse to see that the tumor is still getting bigger. If they have invested time and money, they don’t want to admit it was wasted. We see what we want to see; we remember things the way we wish they had happened. When a doctor is sincerely trying to help a patient, the patient feels a sort of social obligation to please the doctor by improving.
  8. We confuse correlation with causation. Just because an effect follows an action, that doesn’t necessarily mean the action caused the effect. When the rooster crows and then the sun comes up, we realize it’s not the crowing that made the sun come up. But when we take a pill and then we feel better, we assume it was the pill that made us feel better. We don’t stop to think that we might have felt better for some other reason. We jump to conclusions like the man who trained a flea to dance when it heard music, then cut the flea’s legs off one by one until it could no longer dance and concluded that the flea’s organ of hearing must be in its legs!

So there are lots of ways we can get it wrong. Luckily, there’s a way we can eventually get it right: by scientific testing. There’s nothing mysterious or complicated about science: it’s just a toolkit of common sense ways to test things. If you believe you’ve lost weight and you step on the scale to test your belief, that’s science. If you think you have a better way to grow carrots and you test your idea by planting two rows side by side, one with the old method and one with the new method, and see which row produces better carrots, that’s science. To test medicines, we can sort a large number of patients into two equal groups and give one group the treatment we’re testing and give the other group an inert placebo, like a sugar pill. If the group that got the active treatment does significantly better, the treatment probably really works.

Jacqueline Jones was a 50 year old woman who had suffered from asthma since the age of 2. She read about a miraculous herbal treatment that cured a host of ailments including asthma. She assumed the information was true, because it included lots of testimonials of people who had used it and were able to stop taking their asthma medications. They KNEW it worked. They had SEEN it work. Sick of the side-effects of conventional drugs, Jacqueline stopped using her three inhalers, steroids and nebulizer, and took the herbal supplement instead. Within two days she was in the hospital.

I had a massive asthma attack. I was seriously ill in hospital for six weeks, during which I developed pleurisy. I couldn’t breathe and my lungs were so sensitive that even touching the area on the outside felt like someone was kicking me.

All those people who said that herbal remedy had cured their asthma got it wrong. Asthma symptoms fluctuate. Maybe their symptoms would have improved anyway. Whatever the reason, the remedy had not been tested scientifically and it was not effective for treating asthma, and believing those testimonials almost cost Jacqueline her life.

The next time a friend enthusiastically recommends a new treatment, stop and remember that they could be wrong. Remember Jacqueline Jones. Remember George Washington. Sometimes we get it wrong.

This article was republished with the kind permission of Harriet Hall, the SkepDoc.

When nightmares intrude into waking life

0

The Skeptic’s Lead Editor, Chris French, has co-authored a paper on sleep paralysis with Julia Santomauro, published in Issue 22 (August 2009) of The Psychologist. The article has also gained attention internationally from Wired, Mind Hacks, Medindia and ekschi.

If you’ve never heard of sleep paralysis you’re lucky, though “it appears that up to 50 per cent of the population will experience sleep paralysis in one form or another at least once in their lifetime, and some people experience it far more often than that”.

It’s easy to understand why reports of alien abductions are often associated with the phenomenon as “Attacks often involve feelings of intense fear, terror, bliss, joy, anger, and feelings of dying or imminent death. False awakenings are also commonly reported. The individual believes that they have awoken and that the episode is over, only to discover that they are still in fact asleep.”

“In addition, the individual might experience hallucinations. In a sample of 254 college students who had experienced sleep paralysis at least once (Cheyne et al., 1999), 75 per cent had concurrently experienced body paralysis and hallucinations.”

The full article and an interesting list of references are available from The Psychologist through the link above but if you have suffered from sleep paralysis or similar symptoms yourself, the research team would very much like to hear your experiences.

Beware the spinal trap

0

Image: Keep libel laws out of scienceThis is an edited version of an article published in The Guardian for which Singh is being personally sued for libel by the British Chiropractic Association.

Click here for the latest news about the case from Sense About Science or view their Twitter updates here.


Some practitioners claim it is a cure-all, but the research suggests chiropractic therapy has mixed results – and can even be lethal, says Simon Singh.


You might be surprised to know that the founder of chiropractic therapy, Daniel David Palmer, wrote that “99% of all diseases are caused by displaced vertebrae”. In the 1860s, Palmer began to develop his theory that the spine was involved in almost every illness because the spinal cord connects the brain to the rest of the body. Therefore any misalignment could cause a problem in distant parts of the body.

In fact, Palmer’s first chiropractic intervention supposedly cured a man who had been profoundly deaf for 17 years. His second treatment was equally strange, because he claimed that he treated a patient with heart trouble by correcting a displaced vertebra.

You might think that modern chiropractors restrict themselves to treating back problems, but in fact some still possess quite wacky ideas. The fundamentalists argue that they can cure anything, including helping treat children with colic, sleeping and feeding problems, frequent ear infections, asthma and prolonged crying – even though there is not a jot of evidence.

I can confidently label these assertions as utter nonsense because I have co-authored a book about alternative medicine with the world’s first professor of complementary medicine, Edzard Ernst. He learned chiropractic techniques himself and used them as a doctor. This is when he began to see the need for some critical evaluation. Among other projects, he examined the evidence from 70 trials exploring the benefits of chiropractic therapy in conditions unrelated to the back. He found no evidence to suggest that chiropractors could treat any such conditions.

But what about chiropractic in the context of treating back problems? Manipulating the spine can cure some problems, but results are mixed. To be fair, conventional approaches, such as physiotherapy, also struggle to treat back problems with any consistency. Nevertheless, conventional therapy is still preferable because of the serious dangers associated with chiropractic.

In 2001, a systematic review of five studies revealed that roughly half of all chiropractic patients experience temporary adverse effects, such as pain, numbness, stiffness, dizziness and headaches. These are relatively minor effects, but the frequency is very high, and this has to be weighed against the limited benefit offered by chiropractors.

More worryingly, the hallmark technique of the chiropractor, known as high-velocity, low-amplitude thrust, carries much more significant risks. This involves pushing joints beyond their natural range of motion by applying a short, sharp force. Although this is a safe procedure for most patients, others can suffer dislocations and fractures.

Worse still, manipulation of the neck can damage the vertebral arteries, which supply blood to the brain. So-called vertebral dissection can ultimately cut off the blood supply, which in turn can lead to a stroke and even death. Because there is usually a delay between the vertebral dissection and the blockage of blood to the brain, the link between chiropractic and strokes went unnoticed for many years. Recently, however, it has been possible to identify cases where spinal manipulation has certainly been the cause of vertebral dissection.

Laurie Mathiason was a 20-year-old Canadian waitress who visited a chiropractor 21 times between 1997 and 1998 to relieve her low-back pain. On her penultimate visit she complained of stiffness in her neck. That evening she began dropping plates at the restaurant, so she returned to the chiropractor. As the chiropractor manipulated her neck, Mathiason began to cry, her eyes started to roll, she foamed at the mouth and her body began to convulse. She was rushed to hospital, slipped into a coma and died three days later. At the inquest, the coroner declared: “Laurie died of a ruptured vertebral artery, which occurred in association with a chiropractic manipulation of the neck.”

This case is not unique. In Canada alone there have been several other women who have died after receiving chiropractic therapy, and Edzard Ernst has identified about 700 cases of serious complications among the medical literature. This should be a major concern for health officials, particularly as under-reporting will mean that the actual number of cases is much higher.

If spinal manipulation were a drug with such serious adverse effects and so little demonstrable benefit, then it would almost certainly have been taken off the market.


Simon Singh is a science writer in London and the co-author, with Edzard Ernst, of Trick or Treatment? Alternative Medicine on Trial.

The Open Habitat Project

0

Background information

As part of the development of our internet presence The Skeptic Magazine is collaborating with the Open Habitat Project in order to promote science and scepticism within the UK, to an online audience.

The Open Habitat is a creation within the virtual world of Second Life which seeks to create a constructive environment in which visitors can explore and deepen their understanding of people and the world in which we live, through the application of reason, exposure to the sciences, art, philosophy and critical thinking, based on the principle of mutual respect.

The dedicated and talented team of Open Habitat managers believe that ignorance is the main problem afflicting societies and seek to help people to learn or relearn the basics in science, philosophy and critical thinking in order to help them make informed decisions and to think more critically.

The Open Habitat Project began its life as part of a JISC funded Oxford University programme for exploring Multi User Virtual Reality Environments (MUVEs) for educational purposes.

Events and Activities

The Open Habitat hosts a number of regular events relating to science, philosophy and psychology, however the first event hosted in collaboration with The Skeptic was the premiere screening of the exclusive interview conducted with James Randi. This event, hosted at 9pm GMT on Saturday 1st August, allowed 80 individuals to watch the interview on six large screens, in a seating area built specifically for The Skeptic by Maria Hume (a member of the Open Habitat team). It was also be possible to discuss the interview live with the other people seated in the viewing area.

The free screening filled the virtual cinema more than twice over, and attracted comments such as:

  • Interesting interview – thanks
  • Thanks very much indeed – he’s a great guy and a great voice for the cause – really enjoyed it
  • Hey thanks for having programs like this!
  • Thanks very much. Very nice. I will watch it again in a moment
  • I need to learn more about the Skeptic Magazine
  • Thank you for all your help.  I am looking forward to attending many of your sessions
  • Thanks for the wonderful video!!
  • I have to go, but I’d like to hear more about the various interesting things you seem to be doing at some point

The Open Habitat and Sense About Science

The Open Habitat runs weekly psychoanalysis and evolutionary psychology sessions, in addition to philosophy discussions and reading groups each Sunday. The sessions are aimed at individuals of all knowledge levels, so it is definitely worth signing up (for free) and getting involved.

How to find the Open Habitat and its events

In order to find the Open Habitat, you will need to sign up to a free Second Life account here. When signing up, it is possible to use any first name but you can only choose from a limited selection of surnames. You will then need to download the Second Life program and install it on your computer. The client program is available for Windows, Mac and Linux platforms. In order to run the program, you will need a broadband internet connection and a relatively good graphics card (Second Life does still run with some integrated graphics cards).

Once installed, open the Second Life client and sign in. If you are using Second Life for the first time, you will immediately enter a ‘holding’ area from which you can then transport into the main Second Life ‘world’. Once in the full world, open the World Map using the ‘View > World Map (Ctrl+M)’ menu. Search for “Open Habitat” and when this is found, click ‘Teleport’.

Second Life Map

The Open Habitat occupies quite a substantial amount of land so it may initially be a little confusing to navigate, espeically if you are new to 3D vitrual envirnments or games, however this is quickly overcome. Similarly, developing the ability to move around in a vaguely coordinated manner can take a little bit of time, but please do persevere! The Skeptic’s area is quite clearly visible from the air, as seen below.

The Skeptic in Second Life

The Open Habitat Project’s website and contact details are here, however queries about The Skeptic’s involvement with Second Life and its collaborative events can be directed to Mark Williams at digest [at] skeptic.org.uk.