Home Blog Page 101

Werewolves

 

A werewolf attacks a man
Hans Baldung Grien
From Die Emeis (1516)

Guest Contributor: Deborah Hyde

Johann Geiler von Kaisersberg (1445-1510), the ‘Doctor Keisersperg’ of the caption above, was a Swiss preacher who gave a series of Lenten sermons in 1508 which were transcribed and published as Die Emeis (The Ant Colony). The 1516 edition of this collection benefited from the addition of woodcuts attributed to Hans Baldung Grien, including this one, of a werewolf attacking a man.

The fifteenth and sixteenth centuries saw a peaking of interest in werewolfism in some parts of mainland Europe. Authorities such as Jean Bodin and Henri Boguet teased out the details and logic of the witches’ pact, and whether the devil actually had power to really change men into beasts. Charges of werewolfism at this time, therefore, were associated with witchcraft and heresy.

But this was the era of the Reformation and Counter-Reformation, with its attendant paranoia, scapegoating and bloodshed. ‘Werewolves’ tended to be found in more isolated, mountainous areas, where there were likely to be both religious independents and real wolves.

Of the several werewolf trial records that remain today, we can discern a motley parade of political scapegoats, unpopular villagers, torture victims and real psychopaths.

RIP Hitch

0

Anthony Burns ruminates on Hitch’s persistent preoccupations, and wonders aloud about that supposed move from left to right

For a lot of people, their first love is what they’ll always remember. For me it’s always been the first hate, and I think that hatred… is a terrific way of getting you out of bed in the morning and keeping you going. If you don’t let it get out of hand, it can be canalized into writing. In this country where people love to be non-judgmental when they can be, which translates as, on the whole, lenient, there are an awful lot of bubble reputations floating around that one wouldn’t be doing one’s job if one didn’t itch to prick.” 1

The UK was never really going to be big enough to sustain a freewheeling personality like Christopher Hitchens. By nature he was a big dog looking for other big dogs to bark at, and as a man in possession of the intellectual means to make a name for himself doing it, he needed causes, arguments, and opponents large enough to energise and sustain those means, lest his appetites – the booze, the fags, the need for company – devoured him.

Hitchens moved to the United States in 1981, settling into the Washington DC establishment (after conquering New York) with regular gigs at The Nation and Vanity Fair. He found in the US a more open, less stuffy society that would better fit his polemical style, for as he put it, he did not have to pass as many approval tests in the United States as he did in Britain. The furore in Britain over The Satanic Verses also contributed to his belief that Britain was not going to prove a sufficient bulwark for Enlightenment values in the face of religious medievalism, whereas he believed that the United States might.

Appreciating the greater freedom the US gave him did not make him blind to the grip that religion has on social mores and politics there, where even the most egregious of religious charlatans and confidence tricksters are still accorded dutiful respect by a compliant media, regardless of the nonsense they speak. So the visceral thrill of watching Hitchens deliver a massive ‘Hitchslap’ to the nation on the subject of Rev. Jerry Falwell’s death in 2007 was something to behold. What made Hitchens’ decimation of Falwell’s character and legacy so memorable was the trademark delivery of a series of remarks shorn of all of the polite equivocation and false piety that usually attend the deaths of American figures. Rude and shocking it may have been, but also necessary and bracing when discussing the impact and legacy of a destructive force in American life that held homosexuals and abortion rights supporters partially responsible for the September 11th terrorist attacks.

Hitch eventually transmogrified into a rare thing in America, a public intellectual and man of letters possessed of sufficient wit and panache to cut through the sound and fury of a national media that has no place for considered intellectuals or detail driven policy wonks in their debates. He was able to introduce elements of both by sandwiching them between entertaining bon-mots and stylish takedowns, resulting in a rude, clever and extremely quotable personal style that made him ideally suited to the culture of American cable news where talk-show hosts lined up to book him as a guest despite the inherent danger of being effortlessly upstaged. Hitch’s command of language and argument in debate furnished him with a comfortable living to supplement his earnings from writing. It commanded him a following amongst the younger generation who enjoyed his savage wit and erudition and who reposted “Hitchslaps” on YouTube and many other corners of the social media, boosting his profile well beyond that of a jobbing hack or writer.

However, this profitable love of argument also created a danger that he might one day become an exaggerated stereotype of himself, a mere contrarian saying rude things on television to drive up book sales rather than a purpose driven contrarian always with a point to make. Critics pointed out that Hitchens often over-egged the pudding with the take-downs of his opponents, sometimes so strongly wording his invective that the point he was trying to make became buried under the rubble of his own locutive onslaught.

Hitchens did not always wear his learning as lightly as he could, fond in his writing and in person of letting it be known that he read very widely and very well. He loved the patronage of the literary and political establishment first in the United Kingdom and then the United States, actively seeking out their company and endorsement, and therefore, and in his own way, Hitch could be as establishment as any of the establishment figures he professed to despise, perhaps a going concern for any celebrated radical who hangs around select media and political circles long enough. But at least his practice of putting his neck on the line by visiting many of the lethal hotspots he talked and wrote about while they were still hot displayed a courage and conviction beyond some of the armchair warriors with whom he was sometimes unfavourably compared.

Hitchens unreservedly described himself as a man of the left throughout his career; he lamented the demise of a “global international working class movement” while continuing to believe in the Marxist dialectic and the materialist conception of history. However, he also doubted if there was even a viable socialist alternative to market capitalism any more and celebrated the demise of the Soviet Union and the communist dictatorships it propped up, believing the collapse “resulted in a huge release of human energy and creativity underlining the aspect of human nature that is incompatible with dictatorship and human slavery” 2

This continuing commitment to Marxist perspectives made his falling out with left-wing friends and colleagues over the Iraq War particularly discordant and bitter. Some of his strident responses to left-wing opposition to the war would seem to fit his life-long tendency to rebel against his oldest ideas, positions and arguments, so as to eventually take up argument against everybody, even himself. But it would be a mistake to believe that Hitchens’ opposition to the movement against the war was merely that of a reflexive contrarian and agitator, or that he went from being a man of the left to a man of the right (his erstwhile new friendships with Paul Wolfowitz and Richard Perle notwithstanding).

Hitchens always sided with, or saw himself siding with, the people who are against tyranny and oppression, and he saw supporting the Iraq War as an extension of that principle, whether or not the coalition forces were themselves acting in a manner that seemed more consistent with that of their enemies than the values they publicly espoused. In his final interview with Richard Dawkins in the New Statesmen, Hitch stated that he “had one consistency, which is [being] against the totalitarian – on the left and on the right. The totalitarian, to me, is the enemy.” 3 In an earlier interview he had said:

“The realization that American power could and should be used for the defence of pluralism and as a punishment for fascism came to me in Sarajevo a year or two later… That was an early quarrel between me and many of my Nation colleagues, and it was also the first time I found myself in the same trench as people like Paul Wolfowitz and Jeane Kirkpatrick: a shock I had to learn to get over.” 4

If many on the left fell out with Hitchens for his trenchant defence of the Iraq war, they couldn’t help but support his criticism of the Bush administration over the treatment of suspected terrorists delivered into the hands of US forces by ‘extraordinary rendition’ and other Orwellian terms for kidnap and indefinite detention. His decision to allow himself to be voluntarily water –boarded and then starkly describing the experience afterwards as torture confused some on the American right who had become convinced that Hitch had made a permanent transition from the left to the right. Their realisation that Hitch was not their man at all, especially in relation to the place of religion in politics and society, was hastened when he joined the ranks of the ‘New Atheists’ and published God is not Great, then embarked on a world-wide publicity tour to take on all comers in a series of high profile debates, staking out a claim to perhaps be the most visible and effective public atheist since Bertrand Russell.

He wasn’t jumping on the bandwagon when he joined Dawkins and Harris in releasing a bestseller that made out a case against organised religion. Hitchens had been making specific arguments against the pernicious effects of religion throughout his career, whether describing the poison that religion had stirred into the national and ethnic boiling pots in Bosnia and the Occupied Territories, or taking on celebrated religious figures in the forms of Mother Theresa and the aforementioned Jerry Falwell.

As one of the famous ‘Four Horsemen of the Apocalypse’ (a title he was instrumental in devising) he offered his own unique perspective on how religion poisons everything”, describing himself as an anti-theist implacably opposed to the tyrannies of religion. Hitchens thought religion to be false and dangerous, but not trivial, and therefore worthy of his opprobrium. Hitch made numerous contributions to the sceptical discourse on religion, including, “That which can be asserted without evidence, can be dismissed without evidence”, a famous response directed at various mantras ranging from faith-healing to Creationism that claim that it is up to the non-believers and sceptics to prove their fantastical religious claims wrong. His rabble-rousing instincts, ideal for a public platform, dovetailed nicely with the more studious approach of a Dawkins or a Dennett. Up on stage he reduced most of his religious opponents to matchsticks, relying on a remarkable memory that could draw on vast repositories of knowledge to illustrate deeper understanding of the subject at hand than his religious opponents, despite religion being, in effect, their specialist subject.

Here is a memorable quote from one of his last public debates with a religious believer (in this case Tony Blair):

“What we have here, and picked from no mean source, is the distillation of precisely what is twisted and immoral in the faith mentality, its essential fanaticism, its consideration of human beings as a source of raw material and its fantasy of purity. Once you assume a creator and a plan it makes us objects in a cruel experiment whereby we are created sick and commanded to be well. And over us to supervise this, is installed a celestial dictatorship, a kind of divine North Korea, greedy for uncritical praise from dawn to dusk and swift to punish the original sins with which it tenderly gifted us in the very first place.” 5

When diagnosed with a terminal form of cancer at the height of his fame and influence, Hitch decided not to slide quietly off the stage but instead set out to publicly document the journey of his probable early demise. In interviews he forthrightly described the inexorable encroachment of cancer, the suffering and indignity caused by treatments that level the status of every cancer sufferer to that of the other, and, memorably, how he was facing up to impending death and extinction. In 2010 Hitch delivered a master class of prose on the subject of his cancer that contained all the classic virtues found in his best writing, being rigorous in its examination, compelling in its narrative, and serious in the lessons it provided all of us on how a lively, powerful and disbelieving mind can make a determined and unsentimental preparation for death. 6

Hitchens appeared to live by one of Mark Twain’s maxims, that “Sacred Cows make the best hamburger”, making the impact of his sheer hard work in producing continuous output for decades, his fearlessness in prose and in person to cause offence or make powerful enemies, and his powerful, sustained arguments that hit straight through to the heart of much that troubles humanity.

Hitch’s unique voice was unmistakeable, irreplaceable and part of our common cultural heritage. He will be greatly missed by those in the skeptic community and far beyond.

 

Author

Anthony Burns is a skeptical humanist (including being sceptical of humanism for its own good) and a part-time writer, blogger and occasional radio host on all things that provide grist to the mill. Particularly enjoys following the life and times of the discontents and disreputables that remake the world.

You can follow him on twitter at @TheOriginalApe

Footnotes

1 C-Span’s Booknotes, 2003

2 Newsnight interview, November 2010

3 Interview in the New Statesman, 13th December 2011

4 Interview on the History News Network, December 23rd 2003

5 Munk Debate, Toronto, November 27th 2010

6 Vanity Fair, October 2010

Can You Catch Morgellons from the Internet?

Natasha Byrne investigates the role of a very modern medium in the proliferation of a very modern condition

It’s enough to make your skin crawl: relentlessly itchy skin, insect-like crawling and biting sensations, coloured fibres sprouting from open weeping sores and debilitating ‘brain fog’. Sound like science fiction? Not to over 14,000 families worldwide (Savely & Stricker, 2010) who have claimed to suffer from the controversial disease of ‘Morgellons’ – a highly distressing condition with strange symptoms and, as yet, an unknown cause. Many sufferers believe that they are infected with a mysterious parasite but other speculations range from alien creatures, bio-terrorism and nanotechnology to a by-product of genetically modified food. However, much of the medical community dispute that Morgellons exists at all and point to a diagnosis of Delusional Parasitosis (DP): a psychiatric condition where sufferers believe they are infested by organisms that are not actually there (Gieler & Harth, 2008). So if Morgellons really is a delusion rather than a parasite, then does that mean that delusions can be infectious?

In 2001, Mary Leitao became concerned when her young son developed sores around his mouth that felt like they were crawling with bugs.  On closer inspection with a toy microscope, she was astonished to find strange coloured fibres that appeared to be emerging from his wounds. Taking her son to several doctors, alarm bells began to ring for Leitao when not one would believe that there was anything unusual about his condition, telling her repeatedly that the ‘fibres’ must have come from their clothes (DeVita-Raeburn 2007; Elkan, 2007). Determined to find an answer, Leitao searched the internet and found an historical account of a peculiar disease. ‘Morgellons’ was first described in the 1670s by an English physician, Sir Thomas Brown, when he observed strange dark hairs sprouting from the backs of young children. These were later drawn by Dr. Michel Etmuller as a type of parasite with long tails (Kellett, 1935). Leitao felt sure that this is what had afflicted her son and so established the Morgellons Research Fund (MRF; no longer active) and website to research the disease, fight for recognition, and serve as a register for those who also believed they had the mysterious disease.

The plight of Morgellons sufferers caught the attention of the media when a pharmacologist, Randy Wymore and a paediatrician, Rhonda Casey, analysed fibre specimens from a Morgellons patient and failed to match them to any common environmental substances (Elkan, 2007). Thousands of registrations to the MRF followed the media coverage, mostly from California, Florida and Texas but also in all US states and 45 countries worldwide. Sufferers usually report open and slow healing skin lesions, intense itchiness and sensations of crawling insects, as well as the presence of coloured fibres or granules protruding from their sores (Fair, 2010). Other common symptoms include joint and muscle pain, sleep disturbances and cognitive impairment known by the community as ‘brain fog’ (Savely et al, 2006).

Growing media interest and a proliferation of the condition prompted some research, but studies produced ambiguous results. A study by Virginia Savely and Raphael Stricker (2010) found an unusually high number of tick-borne diseases in a sample of 122 Morgellons patients and 96.8% tested positive or suspect for Lymes disease, a bacterial infection. Conversely, a report on the MRF website (Stricker, Savely, Zaltsman & Citovsky, 2007) indicated that Argobacterium, bacteria used for making genetically modified crops, was present in skin samples from five patients but not in samples from controls. Another researcher found that DNA from one individual’s fibres matched a strain of fungus (Kilani, 2002).

Speculation from the Morgellons community is rife due to the inconsistencies of lab results and lack of appropriate treatment. Theories posted on online discussion boards include: parasites, “my suspicion is that this is a tiny nematode [flatworm] or similar bug which can live in the small intestine and also periodically moves to the skin” (posted by ‘jeanlong’, 2010); alien species, “These heavy chem trails [chemical trails deliberately sprayed by aircraft] may be dragging these ‘alien’ organisms down to Earth covering everything” (‘NotDelusional’, 2010); implanted nanotechnology, “I think we are being infested with ‘Smart Dust’ technology. It is being done so the whole world can be mapped out and surveiled (sic) 24/7 by those who have control issues” (‘terracer’, 2011); genetic modification, “I think it is possible that foreign DNA has entered the body through GM foods and agrobacterium” (‘2manyfibers’, 2009). The lack of thorough testing and government involvement in what many see as a frightening and debilitating condition has also sparked accusations of a government cover up. Such suspicions could be fuelled by the fact that the medical community remain largely sceptical about the legitimacy of Morgellons as a real disease and instead believe it to be a form of psychogenic illness.

The term psychogenic illness describes symptoms that are spread within a group that have no possible physical cause (Bartholemew & Muniratnam, 2011). In other words, the illness must be psychological. Indeed, many patients who believe they have Morgellons symptoms have been given the diagnosis of Delusional Parasitosis (DP) as the two conditions share some key characteristics (Savely & Stricker, 2010). Both involve an unwavering conviction that an organism has infected the body, despite lack of physical evidence, and attempts to extract it can result in self-inflicted skin damage (Gieler & Harth, 2008). However, the Morgellons community have fiercely rejected this label, possibly due to the stigma of a psychiatric diagnosis (Freudenreich et al, 2010). One group of researchers (Harvey et al, 2009) proposes some middle ground: the similarities to DP may suggest the two presentations are polar ends of the same broad disorder but with reversed cause and effect.  Morgellons has (as yet unknown) physical causes that also produces psychological consequences, whereas DP is psychosomatic. Medical professionals attempting to treat Morgellons have had limited success, but recorded cases have shown improvement with hypnosis (Gartner, Dolan, Stanford, & Elkins, 2011) antipsychotic medication (Freudenreich et al, 2010; DeBonis & Pierre, 2011) and antibiotics (Harvey, 2007; Robles et al, 2008).

In 2008, increasing media attention and pressure from the Morgellons community prompted the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) agency in the United States to fund a $500,000 clinical study to discover the cause of the mysterious symptoms. The results of the study were highly anticipated by the Morgellons community and many expected that they would finally get an explanation and validation of this strange condition. The long-awaited results were published early this year but were not what the community had been hoping for.

From the evaluation of 115 self-diagnosed Morgellons patients, Michele Pearson and colleagues (2012) could find no evidence of a new and distinct physical disease. They failed to find the presence of parasites or microbacteria and the majority of skin lesions examined were either consistent with insect bites, self-infliction or skin, or nerve conditions. Furthermore, fibre samples that were analysed were found to be environmental: the majority most likely from skin fragments or cotton. So, if there were no common physical signs of disease then were there any other shared characteristics that could account for the symptoms?

Drug testing showed that half of the sample tested positive for drug use. Formication (sensations of crawling under the skin) is a well-known side effect of drug use and withdrawal (e.g. ‘cocaine bugs’), which could explain some symptoms. Further to this, around 60% showed some cognitive impairment, 63% presented with clinical somatic complaints and 37% showed co-occurring psychiatric conditions. However, cause and effect relationships are not clear. Although the authors were careful not to rule out Morgellons as a new disease, they also did not reject the similarities to DP and suggested that patients may benefit from treatments that are effective for DP. So if Morgellons really is a delusion then how could it be spread to so many?

Delusions can be formed when ambiguous experiences are misinterpreted (Maher, 1974). Initially, sensations or experiences can become noticeable to the individual because of their intensity, because they cause distress or if they just won’t go away (Kirmayer & Sartorius, 2007). For DP patients, this could refer to a number of sensations such as skin irritation, dermatitis or insect bites as in the CDC study, or perhaps even just contact with something dirty. Attention can become biased toward these sensations and so excessive scrutiny is paid to any other ‘symptoms’ that may be related, such as peculiar markings on the skin. The stress of having strange symptoms or chronic irritation could also have physical effects (Kirmayer & Sartorius, 2007). These can include aches and pains, nausea, worsening of eczema and lethargy. The individual might then try to find an explanation for their experiences, but if the diagnosis given by a professional is not acceptable then they may look elsewhere… and where better to find information on obscure diseases than the internet?

Self-diagnosis from the internet is inherently problematic as information that is presented as fact is often not backed up with credible evidence. The internet can also act as a ‘carrier’ for spreading ideas and memes (an idea or concept that acts like a gene by being inherited, selected and mutated; Lustig et al, 2009; Freudenreich et al, 2010). In the case of Morgellons, it is possible that the internet had a key role in the spread of the condition and allowed one person’s delusion to develop into an outbreak.

Colligan and Murphy (1979, p.82) define mass psychogenic illness (MPI) as “the collective occurrence of a set of physical symptoms and related beliefs among two or more individuals in the absence of an identifiable pathogen”. Although MPIs are not common, there have been some well documented cases in recent history. For example, a rash of seizures amongst elementary school students in America was attributed to mass hysteria (Roach & Langley, 2002). On a wider scale, outbreaks of widespread Koro (unfounded fear of penis, vulva or nipple shrinkage resulting in death) have occurred in West Africa, China and South East Asia (Dzokoto & Adams, 2005). MPIs such as these often appear when there is mounting fear over terrorism, environmental contaminants or epidemics (Bartholomew & Muniratnam, 2011). This is reflective of the emergence of Morgellons, as there was a culture of fear after the 9/11 terrorist attacks as well as rising unease about GM food, chemical pollutants and vaccination scares. All of which had their own associated conspiracy theories and anti-authority movements. It has also been found that a local condition can develop into a MPI given widespread media attention (Dzokoto & Adams, 2005; Halvorson, Crooks, Lahart, & Farrell, 2008). With Morgellons, 75% of the symptoms in the CDC samples began to emerge on or after 2002; at the same time that the MRF and online community became established (Pearson et al., 2012). Undoubtedly, media attention brought the concept of Morgellons into the public’s awareness. From this, individuals who identified with the symptoms could reinforce their self-diagnosis through the social and group processes of the online community.

The internet allows individuals to connect with others who share the same experiences and also receive sympathy and validation of their problems. Once within a supportive community, group processes can abound where individual views are reinforced and exaggerated and the group consensus becomes increasingly polarised amongst pressure to subscribe to popular opinion (Turner & Pratkanis, 1998). Individuals may have to continually fight for public and medical acceptance of their condition and may rely on confirmation biases to do this. That is, accepting and presenting evidence that verifies their beliefs and discounting evidence to the contrary (Freeman et al, 2002). This could mean, for example, that any benign foreign materials that are present in lesions are counted as evidence of parasites or Morgellons fibres. Additionally, identifying as part of a group (e.g. the Morgellons community) may accentuate suspicions toward the motives of other ‘out’ groups (e.g. medical professionals; Tajfel, 1970) and create mistrust and breed conspiracy theories.

As well as social processes reinforcing delusions, many Morgellons discussions are focussed on encouraging rigorous cleaning or treatment rituals that may actually be detrimental to wellbeing and worsen symptoms. Suggestions for ‘treatment’ can include bathing with hydrogen peroxide, having a restricted diet and exhausting cleaning routines with other harsh chemicals. Furthermore, sufferers may isolate themselves for fear of ‘infecting’ others and become more dependent on the online community.  Cycles such as this can lead to strong belief in the delusion and further deterioration (Kirmayer & Sartorius, 2007).

It is possible to see then, how someone may become ‘infected’ with Morgellons over the internet. The media exposure around Leitao’s interpretation of her sons condition could have presented a possible explanation for many individuals’ ambiguous symptoms. Searching Morgellons on the internet can then allow for hyper vigilance and confirmatory biases towards certain symptoms and inclusion into the supportive online community can validate self-diagnosis. Isolation, stress processes and following others suggestions of dubious home treatments could also worsen symptoms and create a cycle of dependence on the community, which may also result in a mistrust of dissenting medical professionals. All of these factors could account for a fixed delusion of infestation. However, it is also important to remember that current lack of evidence for a physical cause does not rule it out. There is the possibility that a proportion of Morgellons sufferers may actually have an unknown physical condition and experimental findings are being confounded by a larger majority experiencing an MPI.

So, beware. Next time you surf the net, you may get more that you bargained for…

Author

Natasha Byrne is a graduate from Goldsmiths University, currently working in a community mental health team.

References

2manyfibers (2009). Interesting Theorie. Retrieved  06/03/2012 from http://www.morgellons-disease-research.com/Morgellons-Message-Board/morgellons-theories-speculations/

Bartholomew, R.E., & Muniratnam, M. C. S. (2011). How Should Mental Health Professionals Respond to Outbreaks of Mass Psychogenic Illness? Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25(4), 235-240.

Colligan, M. J., & Murphy, L. R. (1979). Mass psychogenic illness in organizations?: An overview, Journal of Occupational Psychology, 52, 77-90.

DeBonis, K., & Pierre, J. M. (2011). Psychosis, Ivermectin toxicity, and “Morgellons disease”. Psychosomatics, 52(3), 295-6

DeVita-Raeburn, E. (2007) The Morgellons mystery, Psychology Today, 40(2), 96.

Dzokoto, V. A., & Adams, G. (2005). Understanding Genital-Shrinking Epidemics in West Africa: Koro, Juju, or Mass Psychogenic Illness? Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 29(1), 53-78

Elkan, D. (2007). The itch that won’t be scratched. New Scientist, 195 (2621), 46–9.

Fair, B. (2010). Morgellons: contested illness, diagnostic compromise and medicalisation. Sociology of health & illness, 32(4), 597-612.

Freeman, D., Garety, P. a, Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., & Bebbington, P. E. (2002). A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(4), 331-47.

Freudenreich, O., Kontos, N., Tranulis, C., & Cather, C. (2010). Morgellons Disease, or Antipsychotic-Responsive Delusional Parasitosis, in an HIV Patient: Beliefs in The Age of the Internet. Psychosomatics, 51(6), 453-457.

Gartner, A. M., Dolan, S. L., Stanford, M. S., & Elkins, G. R. (2011). Hypnosis in the treatment of Morgellons disease: a case study. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 59(2), 242-9

Gieler, U., & Harth, W. (2008). Psychodermatology: The Mind and Skin Connection. American Family Physician, 59(4), 287-8.

Halvorson, H., Crooks, J., Lahart, D. A, & Farrell, K. P. (2008). An outbreak of itching in an elementary school-a case of mass psychogenic response. The Journal of School Health, 78(5), 294-7.

Harvey, W. T. (2007). Morgellons disease. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 56(4), 705-6.

Harvey, W. T., Bransfield, R. C., Mercer, D. E., Wright, A. J., Ricchi, R. M., & Leitao, M. M. (2009). Morgellons disease, illuminating an undefined illness: a case series. Journal of medical case reports, 3, 8243

jeanlong (2010). Morgellons Treatment.  Retrieved  06/03/2012 from http://www.morgellons-disease-research.com/Morgellons-Message-Board/morgellons-treatment/

Kellett, C. (1935). Sir Thomas Browne and the Disease called the Morgellons. Annals of Medical History, 7, 467-479.

Kilani (2002). Investigation of Novel Organism Implicated in Morgellons Disease. Retrieved 31/01/2012 from http://www.morgellons.org/docs/clongen1.pdf

Kirmayer, L. J., & Sartorius, N. (2007). Cultural models and somatic syndromes. Psychosomatic medicine, 69(9), 832-40.

Lustig, A., Mackay, S., & Strauss, J. (2009). Morgellons disease as internet meme. Psychosomatics, 50(1), 90

Maher, B. A. (1974). Delusional thinking and perceptual disorder. Journal of Individual Psychology, 30(1), 98-113

NotDelusional (2010) ‘Upper Atmpsphere Bactrium and ???’. Retrieved  06/03/2012 from http://www.morgellons-disease-research.com/Morgellons-Message-Board/morgellons-theories-speculations/

Pearson, M. L., Selby, J. V., Katz, K. A., Cantrell, V., Braden, C. R., Parise, M. E. & Paddock, C. D., et al. (2012). Clinical, Epidemiologic, Histopathologic and Molecular Features of an Unexplained Dermopathy. PLoS ONE, 7(1)

Roach, E. S., & Langley, R. L. (2004). Episodic neurological dysfunction due to mass hysteria. Archives of Neurology, 61(8), 1269-72.

Robles, D. T., Romm, S., Combs, H., Olson, J., & Kirby, P. (2008, January). Delusional disorders in dermatology: a brief review. Dermatology Online Journal. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18713583

Savely, V. R., & Stricker, R. B. (2010). Morgellons disease: Analysis of a population with clinically confirmed microscopic subcutaneous fibers of unknown etiology. Clinical, Cosmetic and Investigational Dermatology, 3, 67-78.

Savely, V. R., Leitao, M. M., & Stricker, R. B. (2006). The mystery of Morgellons disease: infection or delusion? American Journal of Clinical Dermatology, 7(1), 1-5.

Stricker, R.B., Savely, V.R.,  Zaltsman, A. & Citovsky, V. (n.d.) Contribution of Agrobacterium to Morgellons Disease. Retrieved 31/01/2012 from http://www.morgellons.org/suny.htm

Tajfel, H. (1970). Discrimination in Intergroup Experiments. Scientific American, 223, 96-102.

Terracer (2011). ‘Nano Sterility’. Retrieved  06/03/2012 from http://www.morgellons-disease-research.com/Morgellons-Message-Board/morgellons-theories-speculations/

Turner, M. E., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1998). Model of Groupthink. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 73, 210-235.

 

Behaving Like Animals

0

 

Tessa Kendall looks at primatologist Frans de Waal’s work to muse upon the origin of our better natures

Is human nature a beast that needs to be tamed? Should we “throw out Darwinism in our social and political lives”? Or are we naturally altruistic, empathetic and moral?

In Frans de Waal’s new book, The Bonobo and the Atheist, he takes on the thinkers who believe that morality has to be imposed on our brutish natures and catalogues the growing evidence that disproves them.

There is a long history of thought that the natural world is a merciless struggle for survival and that humans decided to live together ‘by covenant only, which is artificial’ (Hobbes), that natural selection is “a Hobbesian war of each against all” and ethics are humanity’s cultural victory over the evolutionary process (Huxley), that civilization is achieved through the renunciation of instinct and the action of the superego – which men are more capable of than women (Freud), that small children have to be trained to be sociable through fear of punishment and desire for praise (Freud, Skinner, Piaget), that moral behaviour is achieved through reason alone (Kant). A step away from this idea but on the same continuum is the idea that we are potentially but not naturally moral (Ridley).

These ideas have their origins in the Judaeo-Christian teaching that morals have to be imposed from above, that in our ‘natural state’ we are unfit for society because of original sin.

What all these thinkers have in common is a kind of dualism between our ‘better angels’ and the beast within, our Dr Jekyll and Mr Hyde. More recently, a lack of understanding of the difference between predation (of other species) and aggression (towards our own species) led to the popular and persistent image of humans as ‘killer apes’.

The idea persists even now, albeit stripped of its religious origins. Richard Dawkins, for example, has said that “we, alone on earth, can rebel against the tyranny of the selfish replicators” (The Selfish Gene) and that “in our political and social life we are entitled to throw out Darwinism, to say we don’t want to live in a Darwinian world”.

Some disagree. Darwin is one of them. He wrote that “any animal whatever, endowed with well-marked social instincts (…) would inevitably acquire a moral sense or conscience, as soon as its intellectual powers had become as well developed, or nearly as well developed, as in man” (Descent of Man). Stephen Jay Gould wrote in 1980 “Why should our nastiness be the baggage of an apish past and our kindness uniquely human?” Philosopher David Hume believed that moral sentiments come from “a tender sympathy with others”.

De Waal agrees with them. He does not believe in any inner dualism, in the need to choose to be moral or to accept moral instruction from above (gods, philosophers or authority figures) because altruism, empathy and morality are innate in us. What’s more, they also exist in other social animals. They are part of an evolved package of behaviours that make it possible for us to be social animals. He calls the idea that civilization and morality are imposed on a violent, immoral, selfish nature ‘Veneer Theory’.

He is not idealising humans or other animals. Conflict is inevitable. It is how and why we resolve or avoid it that matters. He also underlines the difference between humans and some other social animals: “What is so interesting about human prosociality is precisely that it is not of the ‘eusocial’ kind, which promotes sacrifices for the greater genetic good. We, humans, maintain all sorts of selfish interests and individual conflicts that need to be resolved to achieve a cooperative society. This is why we have morality and ants and bees don’t. They don’t need it.”

For many years, De Waal’s claim that other animals display altruism and empathy was ignored or rejected. What his latest book achieves is to put onto a firm evidential basis the fact that the roots of our social behaviour can be seen in other animals. The critical issue is no longer whether animals have empathy, but how it works.

For example, research has shown that moral decisions involve areas of the brain in humans and other animals that deal with emotions and – significantly – the evaluation of others’ emotions. Human morality is “firmly anchored in the social emotions, with empathy at its core” (De Waal, Our Inner Ape). The desire to treat others well comes from altruism which, in turn, comes from empathy.

There is strong evidence in other animals of reconciliation and consolation after conflict – kissing, embracing and grooming for example, to restore social bonds.  They are aware of unfairness – what economists call inequity aversion – which makes good sense in the avoidance of conflict. This has been seen in animals as diverse as capuchins, elephants, canids and corvids. They co-operate and form social ties, both of which improve survival chances – female baboons with the best social ties have the most surviving offspring, for example. Co-operation is strongest in meat-eating animals as hunting requires co-ordination and meat-sharing to provide a reward. Vegetarian animals are much less co-operative because they don’t need to be.

Social animals show gratitude and revenge – remembering the behaviour of others and paying them back. They target their helping, which requires being able to see a situation from another’s point of view. They are able to delay gratification, which shows self-control – a characteristic thought to be only human. Some are aware of the emotional states of others. When VEN cells in humans are damaged there is a loss of self-awareness and empathy; these cells exist in apes, cetaceans and elephants – but not monkeys.

A recent study “highlights the fact that, similar to humans, sensitivity to the emotional states of others actually emerges very young in bonobos and may not require so much complex cognitive processing as has previously been assumed”. Small children comfort other distressed children, even before they have developed the language skills to be instructed to do it.

Part of the problem of resistance against altruism and empathy in other animals is human exceptionalism, the idea that humans are in some way special, set apart. This too has its roots in religion, with humans as God’s special creation, the only creature possessed of a soul. It has persisted for a surprisingly long time in a secular form both in science and the humanities but is slowly being eroded. De Waal has written: “Humanity never runs out of claims of what sets it apart, but it is a rare uniqueness claim that holds up for over a decade. This is why we don’t hear anymore that only humans make tools, imitate, think ahead, have culture, are self-aware, or adopt another’s point of view”.

He also notes that religions developed in countries where there were no other primates have the strongest tendency to set humans outside nature – they have no animal gods or animal-headed human gods.

He acknowledges that humans have more complex and developed social skills; we alone analyse, discuss and codify our behaviour. He says “I am reluctant to call a chimpanzee a ‘moral being’. This is because sentiments do not suffice. We strive for a logically coherent system”.

Human morality and laws show “a move towards universal standards combined with an elaborate system of justification, monitoring and punishment” – for example the Geneva Convention and the Universal Declaration of Human Rights. In animals there is what is called motivational autonomy – they don’t think about why they do something – for example, they don’t make the link between sex and reproduction or between sharing and surviving.

The only possible exception I can think of (so far) is the arts. No animals have been observed making music, art or literature. Some have been seen draping themselves with greenery and flowers so perhaps fashion design predates the arts.

The fact that it is social animals alone who display similar behaviour to ours is the key. It has been suggested by Dawkins that “humans are nicer than is good for our selfish genes”. But we give help roughly at the same level as we need it; tigers are solitary animals who neither need nor give help, for example. Behaving pro-socially makes society work and affords the benefits of social living to the individual.

Being altruistic may make us feel good but feeling good has little survival value in itself, there must be another reason. Is it selfish to behave socially? Are we all really hypocrites? Is it selfish to care for our young, treat others well and to push to the front of the queue?

The big difference is that queue-jumping is a consciously chosen act whereas instinctive behaviour doesn’t involve thought. As De Waal has written, “Imagine the cognitive burden if every decision we took needed to be vetted against handed-down principles.” We may think about our impulses and choose whether or not to act on them but we do not need to learn or be forced to behave well. We often rationalise our moral decisions post hoc.

One section of the book which looks at atheism has been strongly criticized by some atheists, including AC Grayling, who accuse de Waal of being an apologist for religion. De Waal has said “While I do consider religious institutions and their representatives — popes, bishops, mega-preachers, ayatollahs, and rabbis — fair game for criticism, what good could come from insulting individuals who find value in religion?” and “I am all for a reduced role for religion with less emphasis on the almighty God and more on human potentials.”

This is hardly an apology for religion. It’s true that De Waal doesn’t like what he calls militant atheists or personal attacks on individuals who find comfort in their faith. He singles out Dawkins, Hitchens and Harris among others who, as AC Grayling says “aggressively argue the case for atheism”.

De Waal doesn’t think that all religious people are somehow defective, or ignorant or inferior thinkers. As a scientist, he is more interested in “what good it does for us. Are we born to believe and, if so, why?” and “For me, understanding the need for religion is a far superior goal to bashing it”.

I do take issue with De Waal’s speculation that atheism is the result of trauma. This may be the case for some people but others reach atheism through a mental process that leads them to reject belief. Some people never have a belief system in the first place. De Waal’s memories of the religion he grew up with in Holland contain no apparent trauma to explain his atheism and support this theory.

De Waal’s laid-back atheism may not be the ‘right’ kind, which raises the question: is there now only one atheism that is acceptable in public figures? He writes that the enemy of thought and science is dogmatism, whether political, religious or otherwise, because it shuts down discussion and sets up prophets who cannot be questioned. Does every scientist need to sing from the same hymn sheet as the arbiters of atheism (all white, middle class, old men)? Do they all need to be Dawkinses to be acceptable?

It’s important that this small part of the book does not detract from the whole. The lesson is that all of our behaviours are equally part of our nature, they are evolved and not unique to us. De Waal ends The Bonobo and the Atheist with: “Everything science has learned in the last few decades argues against the pessimistic view that morality is a thin veneer over a nasty human nature”.

 

Professor Frans de Waal is a Dutch primatologist working in America where he is the CH Chandler Professor of Psychology and the Director of the Living Links Centre at Yerkes Primate Research Centre at Emory University. He has been named as one of Time magazine’s 100 most influential people. A leading expert on primate behaviour, he has written a series of books on it – including on morality, politics and empathy.

You can read an analysis of Grayling’s highly flawed argument here.

Tessa Kendall is a writer, researcher and campaigner, one of the organisers of London Skeptics in the Pub and Soho Skeptics.

You can follow her on Twitter @tessakendall

 

 

 

 

 

 

Too Much Medicine?

0

 

Back in 1990 I wrote a paper for this magazine entitled Surely there is something in it: The social psychology of healing in which I said the following: “I insist that if you put together mainstream public and private medicine, alternative or complementary medicine, commercially available, across-the-counter remedies, and so on, then you have a healing industry the collective scale of which vastly outstrips whatever it is really capable of achieving. In short, there seems to me to be far too much of it about. And yet the constant message that I am hearing is that we need more of it”.

Despite the great advances in medical science since then, nothing that has happened has made me alter this view. Just in the month prior to penning this I noticed three news announcements that only serve to support my impression that things have not changed. Firstly, Professor Andrew Carr, an orthopaedic surgeon at Oxford University Hospitals, was widely reported as claiming that “Thousands of people could be undergoing unnecessary, risky and expensive surgeries as most procedures have never been subjected to the rigorous testing drugs are required to have”. He added that the benefits could be partly or entirely explained by patients’ strong expectation that their symptoms would improve after treatment.

Hot on the heels of this came the admission by NHS England that millions of people are needlessly sent to hospitals and GP surgeries by the NHS 111 helpline; eighty per cent of referrals to GPs and A&E could be avoided if callers were able to speak to a doctor.

At the same time, it was reported that the prescription of antidepressants in England doubled over the last decade, despite the expansion of non-pharmacological treatment (‘talking therapy’) under the Improving Access to Psychological Therapies programme which is now used by over 900,000 people per year. I should also mention the ongoing scandal of the over-prescription and misuse of painkillers and antibiotics.

In addition to the above, this July NHS England’s Clinical Commissioners published a report with the title Items which should not routinely be prescribed in primary care: A Consultation on guidance for CCGs. This report listed a range of treatments currently prescribed within the NHS without sufficient justification. To the delight of skeptics these included homeopathy and herbal remedies, which the report considered to be of no proven efficacy. But the sums of money currently spent on these are minute compared with the other treatments on the list.

The issue of unnecessary medicine (in all its aspects – investigation, diagnosis, treatment, etc.) – has now become a personal matter for me following a recent call from my GP. Some years ago I had a cholesterol test which was low ‘borderline’ and I was advised that no intervention was necessary. Now my doctor informs me that the NHS guidelines have been modified and the cholesterol reading I had previously now puts me in the range of someone who would be advised to take statins indefinitely.

For me, the possibility that any treatment that I am offered is unnecessary is a risk factor, even if the treatment itself has no lasting adverse consequences. I have seen too many patients in my professional work – not to mention friends and family members (pets included) – undergoing interminable treatments and consuming enormous quantities of pills every day, without any clear indication as to what, if any of it, is of real benefit to them. If nothing else, there are better ways of spending NHS money.

Coincidentally, a relevant and very informative article by Dr Harriet Hall appeared in July on the Science-Based Medicine website entitled Most patients get no benefit from most drugs. The article is mainly about the dilemma of whether or not to take statins and discusses the importance and limitations of the number-needed-to-treat (NNT) statistic – that is the average number of people that it is required to treat before one of them will benefit. As expected the NNT for statins depends on the population the ‘number of people’ are drawn from (i.e. their risk profile – age, prior medical history, current health status, etc.) and how ‘benefit’ is being assessed (e.g. prevention of heart attack). The risk of adverse side-effects (severity and number needed to harm) is something else to take into consideration – in Dr Hall’s opinion these have often been overstated in the case of statins.

Dr Hall refers the reader to the NNT website where one may look up the NNTs of treatments for a various illnesses and conditions, and a website constructed by the Mayo Clinic to assist physicians and patients who are considering statin medication. Incidentally, some of the treatments listed on the NNT website are of no benefit at all and Dr Hall mentions some of these. However she cautions about being unduly alarmed by high NNTs for quite common treatments such as statins; it’s a blunt instrument for informing clinical decisions.

That over-screening, over-testing, over-diagnosis and over- (not to mention ineffective) treatment are a problem that is acknowledged by professionals within the orthodox medical establishment is a testament to their honesty and integrity (can one imagine purveyors of alternative medicine issuing similar warnings about their practices?). In fact several years ago the British Medical Journal launched an initiative with the uncompromising title Too Much Medicine, whose annual conference was held in Oxford this April. The BMJ is also represented at this year’s Preventing Overdiagnosis conference in August in Quebec (mission statement – “winding back the harms of too much medicine”.

So back to my dilemma. As to whether I’ll decide to take statins or not, I’ll let everyone know later this year – if I’m still around.

 

Michael Heap is the chairman of ASKE and is a clinical and forensic psychologist in Sheffield.

Diluting Homeopathy, even further: the campaign to end NHS homeopathy

The announcement on Friday 21st July that NHS England intends to prevent GPs from prescribing homeopathic remedies was greeted with justifiable excitement by, essentially, anyone who prefers NHS resources are not frittered away on pseudoscience. However before we declare this a decisive victory for rationalism, it’s worth understanding where the announcement came from, what it means, and what happens next.

Since 1985, the Department of Health has had the legal duty to maintain the NHS ‘Blacklist’ – the list of over 3,000 products that are not allowed to be prescribed, officially known as Schedule 1 to the National Health Service (General Medical Services Contracts) (Prescription of Drugs etc.) Regulations. Some products are banned because they are not cost-effective, others are blacklisted because cheaper alternatives are available, and some products are on the list because there is no good evidence they are effective.

When we at the Good Thinking Society became aware of the blacklist in 2015, it seemed clear to us that homeopathy fell into a number of those categories. We wrote to the Department of Health highlighting that, as far as we could see, there was no lawful reason to refuse to blacklist homeopathy. After several months of correspondence, they finally agreed to hold a formal consultation on adding homeopathy to the list. For more than 18 months, we have been encouraging them to keep that promise, but Brexit and a General Election have somewhat got in the way.

Meanwhile, in March of this year, NHS England announced a cost-cutting exercise, identifying treatments they deemed to be ‘low-value’ with a view to preventing GPs from prescribing them. Such low-value treatments included, for example, effective treatments which can be purchased over the counter inexpensively (such as paracetamol), as well as other treatments which are wholly ineffective. Given that homeopathy has never been proven to be effective for any condition, we were surprised to see it omitted from the announcement.

Since March, bearing in mind their earlier commitment, we’ve been writing to NHS England and the Department of Health urging them to include homeopathy in the consultation. So when last month’s announcement showed homeopathy had been made part of the review, we were delighted.

Time for Consultation

As promising as this announcement is – and I do believe it is a very significant step forward – it is important to remember that this is a consultation, not a final decision. NHS England have put forward their position: that NHS homeopathy should be stopped because it is “a placebo and a misuse of scarce NHS funds which could be better devoted to treatments that work.” It is a strong statement, and one we wholeheartedly agree with. As of Friday July 21st, that proposal is the subject of a public consultation which will remain open until October 21st.

We can be sure that homeopaths and homeopathy trade groups will be planning to take part in the consultation and will argue that homeopathic remedies should continue to be available on prescription. It is vital that skeptics, rationalist, interested organisations and other members of the public take the time to make their voices heard, and to submit their thoughts on NHS England’s plan to end homeopathy prescriptions. Taking part is simple, and it will take less than five minutes to do, but its importance and impact cannot be overstated.

The Bigger Picture

Although important, the result of this consultation will only apply to GP prescriptions in England, and it accounts for only a fraction of the NHS money spent on homeopathy. In 2016/17, the NHS spent around £92,000 on prescriptions for homeopathy, but the total NHS budget for homeopathy is £5m annually. The rest of homeopathy expenditure is through regional Clinical Commissioning Groups (CCGs), and their direct homeopathy services. Fortunately, the signs are clear that support for homeopathy among those groups is rapidly drying up.
In 2014, there were 31 CCGs in England funding homeopathy – a fact I gleaned having sent FOIs to all CCGs in the country and mapping out those who commissioned a homeopathy service. This was the start of Good Thinking’s NHS homeopathy campaign, which was officially launched at the QED conference in Manchester. The QED attendees were generous enough to raise almost £8,000 to help us run our campaign – funding that provided vital resources for our charity as we navigated our way through the large, complex and bureaucratic structures of the NHS and the Department of Health.

Our data showed that homeopathy funding was mostly limited to the North West, South West and London regions – locations, at that time, of the homeopathic hospitals. Prompted by questions we raised, NHS Liverpool CCG had a public consultation regarding their homeopathy funding, which resulted in their decommissioning the service in June 2016. Four months later, their neighbours in NHS Wirral CCG followed suit. With expenditure having also ended in Halton, Knowsley, and Central Manchester, October 2016 saw an end to NHS homeopathy in the north of England.

In the South West, six CCGs clustered around the Bristol Homeopathic Hospital had spent a combined £299,000 on homeopathy in 2013/14. When the hospital closed its doors in 2015 that expenditure fell to £184,000, with NHS Dorset CCG ending its homeopathy service completely. With figures for 2016/17 pending, it would be a surprise if that expenditure hadn’t fallen further.

The situation in London remains unclear, with the Royal London Hospital for Integrative Medicine (formerly the London Homeopathic Hospital) offering homeopathic remedies to patients seemingly in spite of policies from CCGs in the region not to fund the treatments.

However, there are signs that the national decline in support for homeopathy may be about to happen in London: NHS Camden CCG recently undertook an audit of their patients at the RLHIM to identify any homeopathy spending, and NHS Enfield CCG’s decision to decommission their homeopathy service is expected in the coming months. We are hopeful that other London CCGs will follow this lead, bringing an end to the estimated £3m spent on homeopathy in the capital.

While the consultation on homeopathic prescriptions will not directly affect decisions to decommission homeopathy in Bristol and London, if homeopathy prescriptions are brought to an end it will surely add weight to the calls to end homeopathy commissioning in both regions. All the more reason, then, to ensure that everyone who appreciates the importance of evidence and reason in healthcare takes the time to ensure their views are heard in the consultation.

The announcement of the NHS England consultation was hugely encouraging for UK skepticism and for everyone who has campaigned hard over the years to make this happen, but it is not yet a victory.

However, with a strong response to the consultation from reasonable, rational voices, there’s every chance that the last days of NHS homeopathy are fast approaching.

The Mysterious Wels Catfish

Hayley Stevens interviews a candidate for our notorious lake monsters

The wels catfish can live for at least thirty years and can grow to incredible sizes, with the largest on record having been nine foot and one inch in length.1 This particular fish was found in Italy where warmer living conditions will allow the catfish to grow so well. More typically, a wels catfish will measure in at between 1 and 1.3 metres in length. There have been reports of larger ones being sighted or caught but these are thought to be inaccurate measurements, or sturgeon mistaken for catfish.

It was with these facts in mind that I became very confused while on a recent trip to Lake Windermere and Loch Ness to research lake monster sightings. Numerous people in both areas cite the wels catfish as a popular and credible explanation for monster sightings despite there being no evidence of wels catfish being present in either body of water.

I visited Lake Windermere with Joe Nickell to research the sightings of the lake monster nicknamed ‘Bownessie’ and it was as we spoke to one of the eye-witnesses, a hotelier named Thomas Noblett, that I first heard it suggested that a wels catfish could be the cause of Bownessie sightings.

Noblett had been swimming across lake Windermere early one morning, accompanied by his colleague Andrew Tighe who was in a rowing boat when they report that a mysterious wave came out of nowhere, hitting both swimmer and boat. They both explained that there had been no boats on the lake prior to or during their journey, and that they hadn’t seen anything in the water as it passed behind them both.

Could it have been a wels catfish? Both Thomas and Andrew seemed to think this was plausible because a friend of someone they knew claimed to have once seen the carcass of a wels catfish on the shore of Lake Windermere; also there is a rumour that an aquarium released two catfish into the lake in the not-so-distant past.

This didn’t strike me as convincing evidence that a wels catfish was the cause for the mysterious sightings. I had previously spoken to Dr Ian Winfield who has been studying the ecology and management of freshwater fish in Windermere since 1990 for The Centre for Ecology and Hydrology (CEH) in their Lancaster facility. During our conversation Dr Winfield suggested that if a creature of quite some size is being seen and misidentified as some sort of monster it could simply be a large pike.

Winfield also mentioned that another possibility – that somebody has illegally introduced a catfish into Windermere. This is something Anglers have been noted to do in other lakes; however Dr Winfield was quick to point out that catfish have never been documented in Windermere.

During my visit to Lake Windermere I visited Lakes Aquarium and spoke at length to the Education Officer, Dave Conway, about numerous ideas surrounding the Bownessie sightings and experiences. We spoke about the wels catfish rumour and he explained that he too had heard that an aquarium had released two individuals, but he wasn’t certain that this was true, nor knew where the rumour had come from. Conway then told me that until a year ago the aquarium had owned two wels catfish that were three and five foot in length. The fish had outgrown their tanks and had been donated by the aquarium to a local golf course which had placed the fish in their pond.

Could the rumour of the released catfish have developed from local speculation about where the two catfish at the aquarium had gone?

When we left Windermere and headed towards Drumnadrochit in Scotland to begin our research into the Loch Ness Monster case, I thought we had left any mention of the wels catfish behind. Indeed the species of fish didn’t come up in conversation until the second day of our visit when Joe and I were sitting in the Loch-side home of Steve Feltham, who lives in his van on Dores Beach on a permanent look out for the Loch Ness Monster. He makes money by selling models of ‘Nessie’ to tourists (I have one sitting proudly on my desk). When you hear about Steve you would be inclined to think that he is on a fool’s errand, but once you meet him and start to listen to his story you realise that he is a man who has incredible insight into Loch Ness and the many mundane things that can, and are, mistaken for some sort of beast in the water.

He spoke to us about the usual culprits such as drift wood, large fish, otters, seals and the wakes created by regular boat activity. He told of the numerous times he has been forced to tell an excited tourist that what they’d captured on their mobile phone was actually just the delayed wake of the regular tourist boat heading back to Inverness after visiting the shore near Urquhart Castle.

It all made good sense, and then Steve mentioned that he thought Nessie sightings could be caused by a small number of wels catfish in the Loch and my heart sank slightly. I didn’t know as much about the ecology of Loch Ness as I do about Lake Windermere so I made a note to fact check this claim when I returned to our hotel that night.

Steve reasoned that a wels catfish can live up to one hundred years in good conditions and can grow to be quite large. A mature individual introduced to Loch Ness in the 1930’s – when sightings were at an all-time high – would have only died within the last ten years, and could account for a number of sightings in that time. There was good logic behind this line of thinking, but it would only be logical if wels catfish were physically present in Loch Ness.

Prior to visiting Steve Feltham in his research van we had visited the Loch Ness Exhibition & Visitor Centre. It is one of the most inspirational places I have visited and I would thoroughly recommend that anyone visiting the area takes the time to visit the centre and see the superb work that Adrian Shine and his team have put in place.

Using a mix of lasers, digital projection and special effects, the walk-through tour is insightful, and provides you with the facts about the research into the Loch Ness mystery while also introducing the audience to the bigger picture by touching upon such themes as the ecology, geology and environmental history of Loch Ness. I guess you could say that the Loch Ness Exhibition Centre is an example of how skeptical outreach works best and Adrian Shine and his team help champion a rational approach the Loch Ness Monster case.

When we returned to our hotel after visiting Steve I contacted Adrian to see if he knew anything about the possibility of a wels catfish causing Loch Ness monster sightings.

He commented:

“There is no evidence of the wels in Loch Ness any more than there is any evidence of my favourite (candidate) – sturgeon. Both theories originated as our attention focused more and more upon fish candidates among the reducing possibilities of any real unusual creatures being the cause of sightings. In neither case is there any proposal that breeding populations need be involved. The small juveniles would surely have been caught by anglers and there are problems with food resources in such an unproductive loch.

In the case of sturgeon, a single navigationally challenged individual might enter from the sea, cease feeding and fail to find a spawning partner. It would then return to the sea having caused a few sightings. In the case of the wels which, of course, is a freshwater fish, Dick Raynor’s proposal was that an individual or two might have been introduced by man into the catchment. Their longevity – maybe a hundred years – might then make them candidates for unusual animals. They would not occur naturally in Loch Ness and would only breed above 20ºC. Neither theory makes any particular correlations with what is reported as seen. There are many other explanations for this; they merely address biological possibilities.”

I think it is likely that fish and animals and inanimate objects are often being misidentified as something a bit more monstrous, but I worry that many are filling in the gaps by making leaps of logic: It could be a large fish being misidentified as a monster; a wels catfish can grow quite large; there are rumours of wels catfish in Lake Windermere; thus, it’s probably a wels catfish…

It is possible that a wels catfish could have been sighted and mistaken for a monster in both bodies of water, but there is no evidential basis to this claim. Until wels catfish are shown to exist in these lakes I feel that we should be looking at the species that live in the lake as the most likely candidates for misinterpreted monster sightings.

Let’s not create another monster that may not live in the lakes at all, based on hearsay and hunches – we’ve already got enough of those!

References:
1 Wood, Gerald L. (1983) The Guinness Book of Animal Facts and Feats, Sterling Pub Co Inc

Postscript: The wels catfish that were in the Lakes Aquarium were moved to Brayton Park Golf Club near Aspatria

Hayley Stevens can often be found sitting in haunted buildings or out in the wilderness of her home county of Wiltshire hunting for monsters; but you would be mistaken to consider her a “woo”. Though such a label would have been correct years ago, today she has gained a reputation as a no-nonsense paranormal researcher who tackles claims of paranormal occurrences with a rational approach. Hayley is best known for her work on the Righteous Indignation Podcast. She is also the producer of The Ghost Field Guide Podcast, is the co-founder of Wiltshire Phenomena Research and BARsoc, and the founder and host for Bath Skeptics in the Pub.

Beware the App-aritions!

0
Hayley Stevens gets her smartphone out and goes ghost-hunting

Imagine the scene – a darkened room, the promise of ghosts. Only a candle illuminates the anxious faces of the ghost hunters seated around a table. The atmosphere is tense: “Are there any spirits?” one person asks tentatively. Silence falls for a few seconds and then comes the reply

“I found fifteen places matching ‘spirits’… nine of them are fairly close to you”.

A screen lights up on the table in front of them, and where traditionally a Ouija board and planchette may have sat; a list of locations helpfully compiled by Siri appears. Behold the modern era of ghost hunting, where the smartphone rules.

Admittedly, ghost hunters don’t actually use Siri to hunt for ghosts, but it’s not far from the reality. Modern ghost hunting has never really let go of its traditional roots of the Victorian Séance Parlours, and ghost hunters are often just upgraded Séance participants whose main aim is to talk to ghosts. The only thing that has changed is the medium they use to communicate with them. Traditionally, these were a table used during table tipping, a trance medium, or a Ouija board. Now it’s high-tech gadgets or, in more recent years, smartphone apps. Traditional methods are still often used, but the modern equivalents grow more and more popular every year, partially due to television shows that make ghost hunting with the gadgets and devices seem sexy and exciting.

The default functions of a smartphone can be useful – a torch, a compass, the camera and voice memos, for example. Yet there’s a whole range of specially designed ghost hunting apps just waiting to be downloaded by Zak Bagans wannabes everywhere. Some of them are even branded by popular ghost hunting television shows and advertised to fans at the end of each episode.

Most popular are those that take over the traditional role of gadgets in a ghost hunter’s arsenal. No longer do you need to carry around heavy flight cases full of different meters, dictaphones and photography equipment (with the hundreds of spare batteries and charger cables) when apps like ‘iEMF’ and the default voice-memos app will do the job just as well while being carried around in your pocket with just one cable. Need to set up motion sensors in a room to catch unseen intruders? No problem – there’s an app for that! Need to set up a ‘Trigger Object Experiment’ to see if an item left on its own will be moved by a ghost? Well you’re in luck because there’s an app for that too!

In Volume 23, Issue 2 of The Skeptic I reviewed an app called ‘Ghost Hunting Toolkit’ that offers numerous tools to aid you in your search for ghosts. From a vibration detector which detects whether an object is moving, or an interrogator that offers a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ button that a ghost can touch in response to your questions, to the power detector that requires the ghost to place its finger on the screen to detect how much energy it has – it’s difficult to tell if the developers are mocking the ghost hunters who buy it, or whether they’re dead serious with their claims.

It gets weirder though. ‘Ghost Radar’ turns your phone into a sonar-like screen where blue, red and green dots appear at random to depict where spirits are in relation to you, and shows how strong their energy is. Stranger still is ‘iOvilus’, used to determine the messages ghosts are trying to communicate by reading slight fluctuations in the EM field. It corresponds these fluctuations with a dictionary of words “inputted by scientists” to reveal what the ghost is trying to say. The result is a list of random words spoken aloud by an electronic voice that the ghost hunters will often make fit with the spooky stories they’re dealing with. It’s a beautiful example of confirmation bias in action: believers ignore the words that don’t make sense, and go crazy for those that do, lauding them as evidence of a paranormal entity.

If there’s any doubt that such apps are actually used seriously during ghost hunts, a search for iOvilus or Ghost Radar on YouTube yields dozens of videos of them being used to detect and communicate with the dead.

Ghost detecting devices are often used in a similar fashion, and not as they’re designed to be. When EMF meters don’t find anything interesting they are turned into some sort of a planchette to be activated by the ghost in response to ‘yes’ or ‘no’ questions. You might ask “Did you die here? Make the EMF meter light up for yes”, or “Is your name Elizabeth? Make the device beep to answer yes”. Similarly with torches, the battery cover is removed slightly causing the batteries to hardly touch the connector, and then questions are asked of the ghost in the anticipation they’ll turn the torch on and off in response. As you’d imagine, the battery occasionally touches the connector long enough to make the bulb light-up, and this is taken as a sign of spirit communication. As with the apps, the ghost hunters will often ignore the times the torch or EMF meter doesn’t respond in the way they’d like it to.

New technology is fun and exciting, but it will be apparent to The Skeptic readers that these ghost hunting devices and apps don’t make any progress in the field of paranormal research. In fact they simply make easier the use of pseudoscientific ideas that help ghost hunters with their biased approach to their cases.

So, gone is the glass divination and in its place is the smartphone: it brings nothing new to the table. However, not only is this a biased and illogical method of investigation, it is also unfair and potentially misleading for those who’ve witnessed something they cannot explain, who have turned to the ghost hunters for help in the hope they can provide some extra insight. It’s fine, even if misguided, to sit at home and talk to ghosts through your smartphone apps, but I think it crosses a line when such techniques are used to offer answers to scared or desperate strangers in their own homes.

The promotion of illogical ideas isn’t the only problem created by paranormal smartphone apps though. In 2012, there were international headlines from numerous ghost photos that were created using smartphone apps designed to help create hoax photos – from the ghost of Johnny Junior taken in someone’s front room in Gloucestershire1, to Jack, the Victorian gentleman photographed in the back garden of a family from Barnsley2.

Where hoax photos used to take some skill and time to set up and create, you now simply need an app and a photo upon which to add a ghost. Within minutes you can have a convincing ghost photo ready to prank your friends. There are similar apps for adding alien spaceships into your photos, and even apparitions of the Virgin Mary and Bigfoot too. Yet an increasing number of people are sending ghost photos created in this way to their local newspapers in alarm. The case of the Johnny Junior ghost photo in Gloucestershire is a great example of the harm that these sorts of apps can do.

A Cheltenham resident named John Gore contacted The Gloucestershire Echo to say that the ghost of a baby had been caught on a photo taken in his lounge. He told the paper:

“One of my cats kept scratching at the wall and jumping up in the area and we’re always taking pictures of the cats. When we got it through we were surprised to find the little figure just stood by the sofa. I have never really worried or had any ghost sightings before. We have had a few strange things happen before, like the TV kept changing channels and turning itself off.”

The paper ran the photo and the story, and it was instantly picked up by international news sources such as The Daily Mail and The Metro. There were problems with the photo though, the first being that the ghost in the photo was a fake created using an iPhone app which – having the same app – I recognised immediately. The second was that John Gore was associating the ghost in the photo with the death of a baby in the house a few years previously. He told the paper:

“I showed it to a lady over the road who has lived here for years. She said somebody who lived in the house before us had a child who died of cot death. It is hard to tell whether it is a boy or a girl, but we have called the ghost Jonny Junior, and it looks to be about a toddler’s age.”

With the speed at which the story spread it was extremely likely that the family who had lived in the house previously would read the story, see the address, and realise that it was allegedly their dead baby in the photo. With this in mind I phoned the journalist at The Gloucestershire Echo and explained how the photo had been created. Although she was resistant at first, I created a similar photo and sent it to her, at which she ran a follow-up story on the hoax. This resulted in an admittance of guilt from a friend of John Gore who had hoaxed the photo to prank his friend, but who had been too scared to admit to it after John Gore had gone to the press.

So we can see that it isn’t the apps themselves that pose a problem, but the context that they’re used in. A hoax photo intended to prank a friend going viral and making someone think that their deceased baby isn’t ‘at rest’ is problematic. Using ghost hunting apps in someone’s home and presenting them as a verified method of communicating with spirits is also a problem and poses numerous ethical implications, yet this is the reality we’re facing and there’s potential for it to get much, much worse.

Perhaps I am being dramatic when I envision the future of paranormal research being dominated with ghost hunters using their phones to first collect evidence of ghosts before then using their Wifi connection to upload it straight to their team websites, but the demand for new and more efficient ways to communicate with and capture evidence of ghosts is always growing, and the potential for new ghost hunting apps built up around nonsense ideas popular with ghost hunters is almost endless.

Perhaps someone will develop an app that enables ghost hunters to conduct experiments by filming television sets with their phones to see if they can capture ghostly faces? Or perhaps an app that enables you to join séances from a remote location? Or an automatic writing app that uses your touch screen keyboard instead of a pen?

Beware the app-aritions. They’re heeeere.

 

 

1 Johnny Junior – http://bit.ly/M0LkeO
2 Jack the Victorian Ghost – http://bit.ly/Mmpl2v

 

Hayley Stevens is a vocal skeptical paranormal researcher and has been actively investigating ghosts and monsters since her teens. She blogs at Hayley is a Ghost and The Heresy Club, and is a co-host for Be Reasonable, a podcast from the Merseyside Skeptics Society.